How animals talk : And other pleasant studies of birds and beast

By Long

The Project Gutenberg eBook of How animals talk
    
This ebook is for the use of anyone anywhere in the United States and
most other parts of the world at no cost and with almost no restrictions
whatsoever. You may copy it, give it away or re-use it under the terms
of the Project Gutenberg License included with this ebook or online
at www.gutenberg.org. If you are not located in the United States,
you will have to check the laws of the country where you are located
before using this eBook.

Title: How animals talk
        And other pleasant studies of birds and beast


Author: William J. Long

Release date: February 21, 2024 [eBook #73009]

Language: English

Original publication: New York: Harper & brothers, 1919

Credits: Produced by Susan E., Jeroen Hellingman and the Online Distributed Proofreading Team at https://www.pgdp.net/ for Project Gutenberg (This file was produced from images generously made available by The Internet Archive).


*** START OF THE PROJECT GUTENBERG EBOOK HOW ANIMALS TALK ***





                            HOW ANIMALS TALK

             And Other Pleasant Studies of Birds and Beasts


                                   BY
                            William J. Long

                               Author of
                “School of the Woods” “Northern Trails”
                     “Brier-Patch Philosophy” etc.

                    Illustrations and Decorations by
                            CHARLES COPELAND




                      HARPER & BROTHERS PUBLISHERS
                          NEW YORK AND LONDON








CONTENTS


    CHAP.                                       PAGE

    I.      A Little Dog-Comedy                    3
    II.     Cries of the Day and Night            10
    III.    Chumfo, the Super-sense               34
    IV.     Natural Telepathy                     74
    V.      The Swarm Spirit                     111
    VI.     Where Silence Is Eloquent            137
    VII.    On Getting Acquainted                175
    VIII.   On Keeping Still                     195
    IX.     At Close Range                       211
    X.      The Trail                            237
    XI.     Woodsy Impressions                   247
    XII.    Larch-trees and Deer                 256
    XIII.   Black Mallards                       266
    XIV.    Memories                             283
    XV.     Beaver Work                          298








ILLUSTRATIONS


    The Old Vixen Lies Apart Where She Can Overlook
      the Play and the Neighborhood                        Frontispiece
    He Flings Out a Single “Haw!” and the Ducks Spring
      Aloft on the Instant and Head Swiftly Out to Sea     Facing p. 20
    A Shadow Moved from the Darker Shadow of an Upturned
      Root—and a Great Buck Stood Alert on the Open Shore            54
    They Stood Tense as They Searched the Plain and
      Surrounding Woods for the Source of Danger                    102
    The Course He Took Was Entirely Different from That
      Taken by the Man Who Brought the Vixen Home                   166
    So Innocent and So Appealing that It is Hard to Keep
      Your Hands from Him                                           192
    His Massive Head Thrust Forward as He Tried to
      Penetrate the Far Distance with His Near-sighted Eyes         252
    At Such a Time My Pond Seemed to Awaken and Shed Its
      Silence Like a Garment                                        270








HOW ANIMALS TALK


I

A LITTLE DOG-COMEDY


Did you ever see two friendly dogs meet when one tried to tell the
other of something he had discovered, when they touched noses, stood
for a moment in strange, silent parley, then wagged their tails with
mutual understanding and hurried off together on a canine junket?

That was the little comedy which first drew my attention to the matter
of animal communication, many years ago, and set my feet in the
unblazed trail we are now to follow. And a very woodsy trail you shall
find it, dim and solitary, with plenty of “blind” spots where one may
easily go astray, and without any promise of what waits at the other
end of it.

One summer afternoon I was reading by the open window, while my old
setter, Don, lay at on his side in the shade of a syringa-bush. He had
scooped out a hollow to suit him, and was enjoying the touch of the
cool earth when a fat little terrier, a neighbor’s pet, came running
with evident excitement to wake the old dog up. Don half raised his
head, recognized his friend Nip and thumped the ground lazily with his
tail.

“It’s all right, little dog. You’re always excited over something of no
consequence; but don’t bother me this hot day,” he said, in dog-talk,
and dropped his head to sleep again.

But Nip was not to be put aside, having something big on his mind. He
nudged Don sharply, and the old dog sprang to his feet as if
galvanized. For an interval of perhaps five seconds they stood
motionless, tense, their noses almost touching; then Don’s plume began
to wave.

“Oh, I see!” he said; and Nip’s stubby tail whipped violently, as if to
add, “Thank Heaven you do, at last!” The next moment they were away on
the jump and disappeared round a corner of the house.

Here was comedy afoot, so I slipped out through the back door to follow
it. The dogs took no notice of me, and probably had no notion that they
were observed; for I took pains to keep out of sight till the play was
over. Through the hay-field they led me, across the pasture lot, and
over a wall at the foot of a half-cultivated hillside. Peering through
a chink of the wall, I saw Nip dancing and barking at a rock-pile, and
between two of the rocks was a woodchuck cornered.

For weeks Nip had been laying siege to that same woodchuck, which had a
den on the hillside in a patch of red clover, most convenient to some
garden truck. A dozen times, to my knowledge, the little dog had rushed
the rascal; but as Nip was fat and the chuck cunning, the chase always
ended the same way, one comedian diving into the earth with a defiant
whistle, leaving the other to scratch or bark impotently outside.

Any reasonable dog would soon have tired of such an uneven game; but a
terrier is not a reasonable dog. At first Nip tried his best to drag
Don into the affair; but the old setter had long since passed the
heyday of youth, when any kind of an adventure could interest him. In
the presence of grouse or woodcock he would still become splendidly
animate, and then the years would slip from him as a garment; but to
stupid groundhogs and all such “small deer” he was loftily indifferent.
He was an aristocrat, of true-blue blood, and I had trained him to let
all creatures save his proper game severely alone. So, after following
Nip once and finding nothing more exciting than a hole in the ground,
with the familiar smell of woodchuck about it, he had left the terrier
to his own amusement.

When speed failed, or wind, it was vastly amusing to watch Nip try to
adopt cat-strategy, hiding, creeping, scheming to cut off the enemy’s
retreat. Almost every day he would have another go at the impossible;
but he was too fat, too slow, too clumsy, and also too impatient after
his doggy kind. By a great effort he could hold still when his game
poked a cautious head out of the burrow for a look all around; but no
sooner did the chuck begin to move away from his doorway than the
little dog began to fidget in his hiding-place, and his tail (the one
part of a dog that cannot lie) would wildly betray his emotions.
Invariably he made his rush too soon, and the woodchuck whistled into
his den with time to spare.

On this summer afternoon, however, Nip had better luck or used better
tactics. Whether he went round the hill and came over the top from an
unexpected quarter, or lay in wait in his accustomed place with more
than his accustomed patience, I have no means of knowing. By some new
device or turn of luck he certainly came between the game and its
stronghold; whereupon the chuck scuttled down the hill and took refuge
among the rocks. There Nip’s courage failed him. He was a little dog
with a big bark; and the sight of the grizzled veteran with back
against a stone and both flanks protected probably made him realize
that it is one thing to chase a chuck which runs away, but quite
another thing to enter his cave while he stands facing you, his beady
eyes snapping and his big teeth bare. So after a spell of brave barking
Nip had rushed off to fetch a larger dog.

All that was natural enough, and very doglike; at least it so appeared
to me, after seeing other little dogs play a similar part; but the
amazing feature of this particular comedy was that Nip had no
difficulty in getting help from a champion who had refused to be
interested up to that critical moment. Through the wall I saw him lead
Don straight to the rocks. The old dog thrust in his head, yelped once
as he was bitten, dragged out the chuck, gave him a shake and a
quieting crunch; then, without the slightest evident concern, he left
Nip to worry and finish and brag over the enemy.

It is part of the fascination of watching any animal comedy that it
always leaves you with a question; and the unanswerable question here
was, How did Nip let the other dog know what he wanted?

If you are intimate enough with dogs to have discovered that they
depend on their noses for all accurate information, that they have, as
it were, a smellscape instead of a landscape forever before them, you
will say at once, “Don must have smelled woodchuck”; but that is a
merely convenient answer which does not explain or even consider the
facts. Don already knew the general smell of woodchuck very well, and
was, moreover, acquainted with the odor of the particular woodchuck to
which his little dog-chum had been laying siege. He knew it at first
hand from the creature itself, having once put his nose into the
burrow; he got a secondary whiff of it every time Nip returned from his
fruitless digging; and he was utterly indifferent to such foolish
hunting. Many times before the day of reckoning arrived Nip had rushed
into the yard in the same excitement, with the same reek of earth and
woodchuck about him; and, so far as one may judge a dog by his action,
Don took no interest in the little dog’s story. Yet he was off on the
instant of hearing that the familiar smell of woodchuck now meant
something more than a hole in the ground.

That some kind of message passed between the two dogs is, I think,
beyond a reasonable doubt; and it is precisely this silent and
mysterious kind of communication (the kind that occurs when your dog
comes to you when you are reading, looks intently into your face, and
tells you without words that he wants a drink or that it is time for
him to be put to bed) that I propose now to make clear. Before we enter
that trail of silence, however, there is a much simpler language, such
as is implied in the whistle of a quail or the howl of a wolf, which we
must try as best we can to interpret. For unless our ears are keen
enough to distinguish between the food and hunting calls of an animal,
or between bob-white’s love note and the yodel that brings his
scattered flock together, it will be idle for us to ask what message or
impulse a mother wolf sends after a running cub when she lifts her head
to look at him steadily, and he checks his rush to return to her side
as if she had made the murky woods echo to her assembly clamor.








II

CRIES OF THE DAY AND NIGHT


The simplest or most obvious method of animal communication is by
inarticulate cries, expressive of hunger, loneliness, anger, pleasure,
and other primal needs or emotions. The wild creatures are mostly
silent, and so is the bulk of their “talk,” I think; but they
frequently raise their voice in the morning or evening twilight, and by
observing them attentively at such a time you may measure the effect of
their so-called language. Thus, you see plainly that to one call the
animal cocks his ear and gives answer; at another call he becomes
wildly excited; a third passes over him without visible result; a
fourth sets his feet in motion toward the sound or else sends him
flying away from it, according to its message or import.

That animal cries have a meaning is, therefore, beyond serious doubt;
but whether they have, like our simplest words, any definite or
unchanging value is still a question, the probable answer being “No,”
since a word is the symbol of a thought or an idea; but animals live in
a world of emotion, and even our human emotions are mostly dumb or
inarticulate. I must give this negative answer, notwithstanding the
fact that I have learned to call various birds and beasts, and that I
can meet Hotspur’s challenge on hearing Glendower boast that he can
call spirits from the vasty deep:


    Why, so can I, or so can any man;
    But will they come when you do call for them?


Yes, the birds and beasts will surely come if you know how to give the
right call; but I am still doubtful whether among themselves their
audible cries are ever quite so intelligible as is their silence.

This question of animal speech has received a different and more
positive answer, by the way, from a man who has spent many years in
persistent observation of wild apes and monkeys. After watching the
lively creatures from his cage in the jungle, attracting them by means
of various fruits and recording their jabber in a phonograph, he claims
to have discovered the monkey words for food, water, danger and other
elementary matters. Moreover, when his phonograph repeats these simian
words the monkeys of another locality seem to understand them, since
they run to the proper dish at the word “food” or show evident signs of
alarm at the word “danger.”

It is doubtless much easier to deny such a conclusion than to prove or
disprove it; but denial is commonly the first refuge of ignorance and
the last of dogmatism, and with these we are not concerned. I do not
know whether Garner claims too much or too little for his monkeys; I
have never had opportunity to test the matter in the jungle, and the
caged monkeys with which I have occasionally experimented are too
debased of habit or too imbecile in their affections to interest one
who has long dealt with clean wild brutes. At times, however, when I
have watched a monkey with an organ-grinder, I have noticed that the
unhappy little beast displays a lively interest in the chitter of
chimney-swifts—a lingo which to my dull ears sounds remarkably like
monkey-talk. But that is a mere impression, momentary and of little
value; while Garner speaks soberly after long and immensely patient
observation.

To return to first-hand evidence: among wild creatures of my
acquaintance the crows come nearer than any others to something
remotely akin to human speech. Several times I have known a tame crow
to learn a few of our words and, what is much more significant, to show
his superiority over parrots and other mere mimics by using one or more
of the words intelligently. There was one crow, for example, that would
repeat the word “hungry” in guttural fashion whenever he thought it was
time for him to dine. He used this word very frequently when his dinner
or supper hour drew nigh, giving me the impression, since he did not
confuse it with two other words of his vocabulary, that he associated
the word with the notion of food or of eating; and if this impression
be true to fact, it indicates more than appears on the surface. We
shall come to the wild crows and their “talk” presently; the point here
is, that if this bird could use a new human word in association with a
primal need, there is nothing to prevent him from using a sound or
symbol of his own in the same way. In other words, he must have some
small faculty of language.

Another tame crow, which an imaginative boy named Pharaoh Necho because
of his hippety-hop walk, proved himself inordinately fond of games,
play, social gatherings of every kind. To excitement from any source,
whether bird or brute or human, he was as responsive as a weather-vane;
but his play ran mostly to mischief, or to something that looked like
joking, since he could never see a contemplative cat or a litter of
sleepy little pigs without going out of his way to tweak a tail and
stir up trouble. At times he would watch, keeping out of sight in a
leafy tree or on the roof of the veranda, till Tabby, the house cat,
came out and sat looking over the yard, her tail stretched out behind
her. If she lay down to sleep, or sat with tail curled snugly around
her forepaws, she was never molested; but the moment her tail was out
of her sight and mind Necho saw the chance for which he had apparently
been waiting. Gliding noiselessly down behind the unconscious cat he
would tiptoe up and hammer the projecting tail with his beak. It was a
startling blow, and at the loud squall or spitting jump that followed
he would fly off, “chuckling” immoderately.

When Necho saw or heard a gang of boys assembled he would neglect even
his dinner to join them; and presently, without ever having been
taught, he announced himself master of a new art by yelling, “Ya-hoo!
Come on!” which was the rallying-cry of the clan in that neighborhood.
He said this in ludicrous fashion, but unmistakably to those who knew
him. Sometimes he would croak the words softly to himself, as if
memorizing them or pleased at the sound; but for the most part he
waited till boys were gathering for a swim or a ball game, when he
would launch himself into flight and go skimming down the road,
whooping out his new cry exultantly. What meaning he attached to the
words, whether of boys or fun or mere excitement, I have no means of
knowing.

After learning this much of our speech Necho took to the wild,
following a call of the blood, I think; for it was springtime when he
disappeared, and the crows’ mating clamor sounded from every woodland.
These birds are said to kill every member of their tribe who returns to
them after living with men, and the saying may have some truth in it. I
have noticed that many tame crows are like tame baboons in that they
seem mortally afraid of their wild kinsmen; but Necho was apparently an
exception. If he had any trouble when first he returned to his flock,
the matter was settled without our knowledge, and during the following
autumn there was evidence that he was again in good standing. Long
afterward, as I roamed the woods, I might hear his lusty “Ya-hoo! Come
on!” from where he led a yelling rabble of crows to chivvy a sleeping
owl or jeer at a running fox; and occasionally his guttural cry sounded
over the tree-tops when I could not see him or know what mischief was
afoot. He never returned to the house, and never again joined our play
or allowed a boy to come near him.

Not all crows have this “gift of speech”; and the fact that one tame
crow learns to use a few English words, while five or six others hold
fast to their own lingo, has led to the curious belief that, if you
want to make a crow talk, you must split his tongue. How such a belief
originated is a mystery; but it was so fixed and so widespread when I
was a boy that no sooner was a young crow taken from a nest than
jack-knives were sharpened, and the leathery end of the crow’s tongue
was solemnly split after grave debate whether a seventh or a third part
was the proper medicine. If the crow talked after that, it was proof
positive that the belief was true; and if he remained dumb, it was a
sign that there was something wrong in the splitting; which is
characteristic of a large part of our natural-history reasoning. The
debates I have heard or read on the “unanswerable” question of how a
chipmunk digs a hole without leaving any earth about the entrance (a
question with the simplest kind of an answer) are mostly suggestive of
the split-tongue superstition of crow language.

Of the tame crows I have chanced to observe, only a small proportion
showed any tendency to repeat words; and these gifted ones are, I
judge, the same crows that in a wild state may occasionally be heard
whistling like a jay, or “barking” or “hooting” or making some other
call which ordinary crows do not or cannot make, and which shows an
individual talent of mimicry. This last, which I have repeatedly
observed among wild crows, is a very different matter from speech; but
from the fact that these mimics learn to use a few English words more
or less intelligently one might not be far wrong in concluding that
every crow has in his brain a small undeveloped nest of cells
corresponding to our “bump” of language.

A closer observation of the wild birds may confirm this possibility.
Thus, when you hear a solitary crow in a tree-top crying, “Haw! Haw!”
monotonously, dipping his head or flirting his tail every time he
repeats it, you may be sure that somewhere within range of his eye or
voice a flock of his own kind are on the ground, feeding. That this
particular haw is a communication to his fellows, telling them that the
sentinel is on watch and all is well, seems to me very probable. There
are naturalists, I know, who ingeniously resolve the whole phenomenon
into blind chance or accident; but that does not square very well with
the intelligence of crow nature as I have observed it; nor does it
explain the fact that once, when I avoided the sentinel and crept near
enough to shoot two members of the flock he was supposedly guarding,
the rest were no sooner out of danger than they whirled upon the
recreant and beat him savagely to the ground.

If you are interested enough to approach any crow-sentinel in a casual
or indifferent kind of way (he will take alarm if you approach quickly
or directly), you must note that his haw changes perceptibly while you
are yet far off. It is no longer formal or monotonous; nor is it
uttered with the same bodily attitude, as your eyes plainly see. You
would pronounce and spell the cry exactly as before (it should be
written aw or haw, not caw, for there is no consonant sound in it); but
if your ears are keen, they will detect an entirely different accent or
inflection, as they detect different accents and meanings when a
sailor’s casual or vibrant “Sail ho!” sings down from the crow-nest of
a ship. Now run a few steps toward the sentinel, or pretend to hide and
creep, and instantly the haw changes again. This time the accent is
sharper even to your dull ears; and hardly is the cry uttered when all
the crows of the unseen flock whirl into sight, heading swiftly away to
the woods and safety.

Apparently, therefore, this simple haw of the crow is like a root word
of certain ancient languages, the Chinese, for example, which has
several different intonations to express different ideas, but which all
sound alike to foreign ears, and which are spelled alike when they
appear in foreign print. To judge by the crows’ action, it is certain
that their elementary haw has at least three distinct accents to
express as many different meanings: one of “all’s well,” another of
“watch out,” and a third of “be off!” Moreover, the birds seem to
understand these different meanings as clearly as we understand plain
English; they feed quietly while haw means one thing, or spring aloft
when it means another; and though you watch them a lifetime you will
see nothing to indicate that there is any doubt or confusion in their
minds as to the sentinel’s message.

Not only the crows, but the wild ducks as well, and the deer and the
fox and many other creatures, seem to understand crow-talk perfectly,
or at least a part of it which concerns their own welfare. Thus, on the
seacoast in winter you hear the crows hawing continually as they follow
the tide-line in search of food. For hours this talk goes on, loudly or
sleepily, and the wild ducks pay absolutely no attention to it; though
they must know well that hungry crows will kill a wounded or careless
duck and eat him to the bones whenever they have a chance. Because of
this dangerous propensity you would naturally expect the water-fowl to
be suspicious of the black freebooter and to be alert when they see or
hear him; but no sooner do you begin to hunt with a gun than you learn
a thing to make you respect the crow, and perhaps to make you wonder
how much or how very little you know of the ways of the wood folk.

Many of the ducks, the black or dusky mallards especially, like to come
ashore every day in a secluded spot under the lee of a bank, there to
rest or preen or take a quiet nap in company. It is a tempting sight to
see a score or a hundred of the splendid birds in a close group, their
heads mostly tucked under their wings; but it is practically impossible
to stalk them, for the reason that the crows are forever ranging the
shore, and a crow never passes a group of sleeping ducks without
lifting his flight to take a look over the bank behind them. What his
motive is no man can say; we only note that, in effect, he stands
sentinel for the ducks against a common enemy, as he habitually does
for his own kind. There is no escaping that keen, searching glance of
his; he sees you creeping through the beach-grass or hiding behind a
bush. He flings out a single haw! with warning, danger, derision in it;
and now the same ducks that have heard him all day without concern
spring aloft on the instant and head swiftly out to sea.

The crows have several other variations of the same cry, expressive of
other matters, which all the tribe seem to understand clearly, but
which are meaningless to human ears. When I imitate the distress-call
of a young crow, for example, I can bring a flock over my head at
almost any time, the only condition being that I keep well concealed.
At the first glimpse of a man in hiding they sheer off, and it is
seldom that I can bring them back a second time to the same spot; yet I
have a companion, one who utters a call very much like mine to ordinary
ears, who can bring the flock back to him even after they have seen him
and suffered at his hands. More than once I have stood beside him in
the woods and fired a gun repeatedly, killing a crow and scattering the
flock pell-mell at every shot; but no sooner does he begin to talk
crow-talk than back they come again. What he says to them that I do not
or cannot say is something that only the crows understand.

It is commonly assumed that they come to such a call because they hear
in it a cry for help from one of their own kind. That is undoubtedly
true at times; for a help-call, especially from a cub or nestling, is a
summons to which most animals and birds instinctively respond. And,
strangely enough, the smaller they are the braver they seem to be. A
mother-partridge has more than once flown in my face or beaten me with
her wings, while “fierce” hawks, owls and eagles have merely circled
around me at a safe distance when I came near their young. In the
majority of cases, however, I think that birds come to a distress-call
simply because the excitement of an individual spreads to all creatures
within sight or hearing, just as a crowd of men or women will become
excited and rush to a common center before they know what the stir is
all about.

In confirmation of this theory, it is not necessary to cry like a
distressed young crow to bring a flock over your head. The imitated
hawing of an old crow will do quite as well, if you throw the proper
excitement into it. Again, on any summer day you will hear in your own
yard the pip-pip of arriving or departing robins. The same call is
uttered by both sexes, at all times and in all places; yet if you
listen closely you must note that there is immense variety in the
accent or inflection of even this simple sound. The call is clear,
ringing, joyous when the robins first arrive in the spring; it is
subdued when they gather for the autumn flight; it is sleepy or
querulous when they stand full-fed by the nest, and most business-like
when they launch themselves into flight, which is the moment when you
are most sure to hear it. A robin utters this call hundreds of times
every day, in one accent or another, and neither the other robins nor
their feathered neighbors seem to pay any attention to it; but when a
red squirrel comes plundering a nest, and the mother robin sends forth
the same pip-pip with a different intonation, then the response is
instantaneous. The alarm spreads swiftly over wood and field; clamor
uprises, and birds of many species come rushing in from all directions;
not because they have heard that Meeko is again killing young robins
(at least, it does not seem so to me), but because excitement is afoot,
and they are bound to join it or find out about it before they can
settle down comfortably to their own affairs.

There is an interesting way by which you may test this contagion of
excitement for yourself. Hide at the edge of the woods or in any other
bird neighborhood in the early morning, preferably at a season when
every nest has eggs or fledglings in it; press two fingers against your
lips and draw the breath sharply between them, repeating the squeaky
cry as rapidly as possible. The sound has a peculiarly exciting quality
even to human ears (twice have I seen men run wildly to answer it), and
birds come to it as boys to a fire alarm. In a few moments you may have
them streaming in from the four quarters of bird world, all highly
excited, and perhaps all ready to protect some innocent nest from snake
or crow or squirrel. Because the response is most electric at the
season when fledglings are most helpless, you are apt to think that
this call of yours is mistaken by mother birds for a cry for help. That
may be true; but be not too sure about it. The fledglings themselves
will come almost as readily to the call when the nesting season is over
and gone.

I have tried that same exciting summons in many places, wild or
settled, and commonly but not invariably with the same result, as if it
were a word from the universal bird language. Once in a secluded valley
of northern Italy I saw a hunter with his gun, and promptly forgot my
own errand in order to chum with him and find out what he had learned
of the wood folk. He was hunting birds to eat. “Those birds there!” he
said, pointing to a passing flock which I did not recognize, but which
seemed pitifully small game to me. Presently I learned that he could
not shoot flying, and was having such bad luck that, he said, the devil
surely had a hand in it. He was a smiling, companionable loafer, and
for a time I tagged after him, watching him amusedly as he made careful
but vain stalks of little birds that seemed to have been made wild by
much hunting. In a spirit of thoughtless curiosity, and perhaps also to
test bird nature in a strange land, I invited the hunter to hide with
me in a thicket while I gave the call which had so often brought the
feathered folk of my own New England woods. At my cry a wisp of birds
whirled in to light at the edge of the covert; the Italian’s gun
roared; and then I discovered that the wretch was killing skylarks.

I have since had many an uncomfortable moment at the thought of how
many lovely songsters may have paid with their lives for that ungodly
experiment; for my companion hailed me as a master Nimrod from the New
World; and when I refused, on the plea of bad luck, to teach him the
call, I heard him give a distressingly good imitation of it. Yet the
experiment seemed to prove that everywhere birds quickly catch the
contagion of excitement; that in many cases they respond to a call
because it stirs their anger or curiosity rather than because it
conveys any definite summons for help or warning of danger.

When you open your ears among the beasts you hear precisely the same
story; that is, certain cries apparently have definite meaning, like
the accented haw of a crow, while others convey and also spread a wild
emotion. Of all beasts, the wolves are perhaps the keenest, the most
intelligent, and these seem to have definite calls for food or help or
hunting or assembly. Such calls are strictly tribal, I think, like the
dialects of Indians, since the call of a coyote is quite different from
the call of a timber wolf even when both intend to convey the same
meaning. A friend of mine, an excellent mimic, who spent many years in
the West, has shot more than a score of coyotes after drawing them
within range by sending forth the food-call in winter; but though he
knows also the food-call of the timber wolf, he has never once deceived
these larger brutes by his imitation of it; nor has he ever seen a wolf
of one species respond to the food or hunting call of another.

Like most other wild animals, timid or savage, the sensitive wolves all
respond, but much more warily than the birds, to almost any
inarticulate cry expressive of emotional excitement; just as your dog,
who is yesterday’s wolf, grows uneasy when you whine in your nose like
a distressed puppy, or leaps up, ready to fly out of door or window,
when a wild ki-yi breaks out in the distance. Indeed, it is easier to
keep a boy from a fire than a dog from a crowd or excitement of any
kind; and the same is true of their wild relatives, though the wariness
of the latter keeps them hidden where you cannot follow their action.
The greatest commotion I ever witnessed in a timber-wolf pack was
occasioned by the moaning howl of a wounded wolf on a frozen lake in
midwinter. It was a cry utterly unlike anything I had ever recorded up
to that time, and every time they heard it the grim beasts ran wildly
here and there, howling like lunatics. Then, when the wounded one grew
quiet, they would approach and sniff him all over; after which some
would sit on their tails and watch him closely, while others circled
about on the ice, using their noses like hounds in search of a lost
trail.

Occasionally, when I have had these uncanny brutes near me in the
North, I have tried to call them or make them answer by giving what
seemed to me a very good imitation of their cries; but seldom has a
howl of mine been returned. On the contrary, the brutes almost always
stop their howling whenever I begin to talk wolf-talk, as if they were
listening and saying, “What under the moon is that now?” Then old
Tomah, the Indian, comes out of his blanket and gives a howl exactly
like mine, but with something in it which I cannot fathom or master,
and instantly from the snow-filled woods comes back the wild wolf
answer.

Likewise, I have called moose in many different localities, and am
persuaded that it makes very little difference what kind of whine or
grunt or bellow you utter, since anything resembling a moose-call will
do the trick if you know how to put the proper feeling into your voice.
After listening carefully to many callers, I note this characteristic
difference: that one man invariably makes the game wary, suspicious,
fearful, no matter how finely he calls; while another in the same
place, with the same trumpet and apparently with the same call, manages
to put something into his voice, something primal, emotional and
essentially animal, which brings a bull moose hurriedly to investigate.
Thus it happens that the worst caller I ever heard—worst in that he had
no sense, no cunning, no knowledge of moose habits, and uttered a
blatant, monstrous roar unlike anything a sane man ever heard in the
heavens above or the earth beneath—was still the most successful in
getting his game into the open. Three nights in succession I heard him
call in a region where moose were over-shy from much hunting, and where
my own imitation of the animal’s natural voice brought small response.
In that time fourteen bulls answered him, all that were within hearing,
I think; and every one of the great brutes threw caution to the dogs
and came out on the jump.

From such observations, and from others which I have not chronicled, I
judge that the higher orders of birds and beasts have a few calls which
stand for definite things, or mental images of things, but that their
ordinary cries merely project an emotion or excitement in such a way
that it stirs a similar emotion in other birds or beasts of the same
species; just as the sound of hearty laughter invariably stirs the
feeling of mirth in men who hear it, or any inarticulate cry of fear
sets human feet in motion—toward the cry if the hearer be brave, or
away from it if he be of cowardly disposition. Yet even among men, who
by civilization have lost some of their natural virtues, the primal
impulse still lives. Like the wolf or the raccoon, the man’s first
impulse is to rush to his distressed or excited fellows. If he turns
and runs the other way, it means simply that his artificial habit or
training has deadened his natural instincts.

In speaking of “man” here I refer to the genus homo, not to the male
specimen thereof. Among brutes most of the natural instincts are the
same in both sexes; they vary in degree, not in kind, and the instincts
of the female are commonly the stronger or keener. Yet I have noticed,
or think I have noticed, this difference: when a cry of distress is
uttered in the woods, the first bird or beast to appear is almost
always a female; but the male is quicker on his toes at a battle-yell
or a senseless clamor.

This last is a personal impression, and cannot well be verified. The
only record I have which might pass for evidence in the matter comes
from my observation of the crows. In the spring many of these
questionable birds indulge their taste for eggs or tender flesh and
soon become incurable nest-robbers; and for that reason I often shoot
them, to save other and more useful birds. The method is very simple:
one hides and calls, and takes the crows as they appear in swift
flight, the number shot being commonly limited to one or two at a time.
And I have observed repeatedly, at different times and in different
localities, that when I use the distress-call of a young crow as a
decoy, the first to appear over the tree-tops is a female. This is the
common rule, with occasional exceptions to point or emphasize it. But
whenever I clamor like a crow that has discovered an owl, or send forth
a senselessly excited hawing, almost invariably the first crow to come
whooping over is a long-winged and glossy old male.

Does it seem to you like thoughtless barbarity on my part to kill crows
in this fashion? Perhaps it is barbarous; I do not quite know; but it
certainly is not thoughtless. One cannot blame the crows for their
taste in eggs or nestlings; but one must note that they destroy an
enormous number of insectivorous birds, and that the harm they do in
this respect outweighs their usefulness in destroying field-mice and
beetles. I write this with regret; for I admire the crow, and consider
him as, of all birds, the most intelligent and the most considerate of
his own kind. I know that it is a moot question whether the crow does
more harm or good, and that some naturalists have settled it in his
favor; but I have too often caught him plundering nests in the
springtime to be much impressed by his alleged usefulness at other
seasons. I think that he may have been once useful in preserving the
so-called balance of nature; but that balance is now dangerously
unequal. The crow has flourished even in well-settled regions, thanks
to his superior wit, while other useful birds have fearfully
diminished, and this at a time when our orchards and gardens call more
and more insistently for their help. Because of his disproportionate
numbers the crow now appears to me, like our destructive and useless
cats, as a positive menace in a country where he once occupied a modest
or inconspicuous place—such a place as he still occupies in the
wilderness, where I meet him but rarely, and where I am glad to leave
him in peace, since he does not seriously interfere with his more
beautiful or more useful neighbors. But we are wandering from the dim
trail of animal communication, which we set out to follow.

The inarticulate but variously accented cries of which we have spoken
constitute the only animal language to which our naturalists have thus
far paid any attention; and doubtless some of them would object to the
use of the word “language” in such a connection. In all matters of real
natural history, however (real, that is, in the sense of dealing at
first hand with individual birds or beasts), I am much more inclined to
listen to old Tomah, who says, when I ask him whether animals can talk:
“Talk? Course he kin talk! Eve’ting talk in hees own way. Hear me now
make-um dat young owl talk.” And, stepping outside the circle of
camp-fire light, Tomah utters a hoot, which is answered at a distance
every time he tries it. After parleying with the stranger in this
tentative fashion, Tomah sends forth a different call; and immediately,
as if in ready acceptance of an invitation, a barred owl glides like a
gray shadow into a tree over our heads. I have heard that same old
Indian use horned-owl talk, wolf and beaver and woodpecker talk, and
several other dialects of the wood folk, in the same fascinating and
convincing way.

One must judge, therefore, that most cries of the day or night have
their meaning, if only one knows how to hear them; yet they constitute
but a part, and probably a very small part, of the animal’s habitual
communication with his fellows. The bulk of it appears to be of that
silent kind which passed between Don and Nip, and which, I have reason
to believe, is the common language of the whole animal kingdom.

To prove such a matter is plainly impossible. Even to investigate it
frankly is to enter a shadowy realm between the conscious and
subconscious states, where no process can be precisely followed, and
where the liability to error is always present. Let us therefore begin
on familiar ground by examining certain phenomena which we cannot
explain, to be sure, but which have been observed frequently enough to
give us confidence that we are dealing with realities. I refer
especially to that curious warning or “feeling” of impending danger,
which is supposed (erroneously, I think) to depend upon the so-called
sixth sense of animals and men.








III

CHUMFO, THE SUPER-SENSE


For the word chumfo I am indebted to a tribe of savages living near
Lake Mweru, in Africa, and am grateful to them not only for naming a
thing which has no name in any civilized language, but also for an
explanation of its function in the animal economy. We shall come to the
definition of the word presently, after we have some clear notion of
the thing for which the word stands. As Thomas à Kempis says, if I
remember correctly, “It is better to feel compassion than to know how
to define it.”

By way of approach to our subject, let it be understood that chumfo
refers in a general way to the animal’s extraordinary powers of sense
perception, which I would call his “sensibility” had not our novelists
bedeviled that good word by making it the symbol of a false or
artificial emotionalism. Every wild creature is finely “sensible” in
the true meaning of the word, his sensitiveness being due to the fact
that there is nothing dead or even asleep in nature; the natural animal
or the natural man is from head to foot wholly alive and awake. And
this because every atom of him, or every cell, as a biologist might
insist, is of itself sentient and has the faculty of perception. Not
till you understand that first principle of chumfo will your natural
history be more than a dry husk, a thing of books or museums or stuffed
skins or Latin names, from which all living interest has departed.

I am sometimes asked, “What is the most interesting thing you find in
the woods?” the question calling, no doubt, for the name of some bird
or beast or animal habit that may challenge our ignorance or stir our
wonder. The answer is, that whether you search the wood or the city or
the universe, the only interesting thing you will ever find anywhere is
the thrill and mystery of awakening life. That the animal is alive, and
alive in a way you ought to be but are not, is the last and most
fascinating discovery you are likely to make in nature’s kingdom. After
years of intimate observation, I can hardly meet a wild bird or beast
even now without renewed wonder at his aliveness, his instant response
to every delicate impression, as if each moment brought a new message
from earth or heaven and he must not miss it or the consequent
enjoyment of his own sensations. The very sleep of an animal, when he
seems ever on the thin edge of waking, when he is still so in touch
with his changing world that the slightest strange sound or smell or
vibration brings him to his feet with every sense alert and every
muscle ready,—all this is an occasion of marvel to dull men, who must
be called twice to breakfast, or who meet the violent clamor of an
alarm-clock with the drowsy refrain:


    Yet a little sleep, a little slumber,
    A little more folding of the hands to sleep!


You will better understand what I mean by the animal’s aliveness, his
uncloying pleasure in the sensation of living, if you can forget any
tragical theories or prejudices of animal life which you have chanced
to read, and then frankly observe the first untrammeled creature you
meet in the outdoor world. Here at your back door, for example, is a
flock of birds that come trooping from the snowy woods to your winter
feast of crumbs. See how they dart hither and yon between mouthfuls, as
if living creatures could not be still or content with any one thing,
even a good thing, in a world of endless variety. Look again, more
closely, and see how they merely taste of the abundance on your table,
and straightway leave it for a morsel that the wind blows from under
their beaks, and that they are bound to have if it takes all winter.
Every other minute they flit to a branch above the table, look about
alertly, measure the world once more, make sure of the dog that he is
asleep, and of the sky that it holds no hawk; then they wipe their
bills carefully, using a twig for a napkin, and down to the table they
go to begin all over again. So every bite is for them a feast renewed,
a feast with all the spices of the new, the fresh, the unexpected and
the adventurous in it.

Or again, when you enter the wilderness remote from men, here is a deer
slipping shadow-like through the shadowy twilight, daintily tasting
twenty varieties of food in as many minutes, and keeping tabs on every
living or moving or growing thing while she eats; or a fox, which seems
to float along like thistle-down in the wind, halting, listening,
testing the air-smells as one would appreciate a varied landscape,
playing Columbus to every nook or brush-pile and finding in it
something that no explorer ever found before. Such is the natural way
of a fox, which makes a devious trail because so many different odors
attract him here or there.

In fine, to watch any free wild creature is to understand the singing
lines from “Saul”:


    How good is man’s life, the mere living, how fit to employ
    All the heart and the soul and the senses forever in joy!


It is to understand also the spirit of Browning, who is hardly a world
poet, to be sure, but who has the distinction of being the only famous
poet who is always alive and awake. Homer nods; Dante despairs and
mourns; Shakespeare has a long period of gloom when he can write only
terrible tragedies of human failure; other great poets have their weary
days or melancholy hours, but Browning sings ever a song of abounding
life. Even his last, the Epilogue to “Asolando,” is not a swan-song,
like Tennyson’s; it is rather a bugle-call, and it sounds not the
“taps” of earth, but the “reveille” of immortality. But we are
wandering from our woodsy trail.

Those who make an ornithology of mere feathers, or who imagine they
know an animal because they know what the scientists have said about
him, see in this instant responsiveness of the wild creature only a
manifestation of fear, and almost every book of birds or beasts repeats
the story of terror and tragedy. Yet every writer of such books
probably owns a dog that displays in less degree (because he is less
alive) every single symptom of the wild creature, including his alleged
fears; and the dog, far from leading a tragic or terror-governed life,
is hilariously disposed to make an adventure or a picnic of every new
excursion afield. Moreover, if one of these portrayers of animal fears
or tragedies has ever had an adventure of his own; if he has penetrated
a wild region on tiptoe, or run the white rapids in a canoe, or heard
the wind sing in his ears on a breakneck gallop across country, or
trailed a bear to his covert, or hunted bandits in the open, or
followed the bugles when they blew for war,—then he must know well that
these unforgetable moments, when a man’s senses all awaken and his
nerves tingle and he treads the earth like a buck in spring, are the
only times in a man’s dull life when he feels himself wholly alive and
a man. That a naturalist should forget this when he sees an alert wild
animal, and deny his dog and his own experience of life by confounding
alertness with fear, is probably due not so much to his own blindness
as to his borrowed notions, such as the “struggle for existence,” the
“reign of terror,” and other hallucinations which have been packed into
his head in the name of science or natural history.

Just to take a walk with your dog may be a revelation to you, as most
simple matters are when you dare to view them for yourself without
prejudice. In fact, what is any revelation or discovery but seeing
things as they are? This daily walk over familiar ground, which bores
you because you must take it for exercise, leaving out of it the fun
which is the essential element of any exercise, is to your dog another
joyous and expectant exploration of the Indes. He explored the same
ground yesterday, to be sure, but all sorts of revolutions and
migrations take place overnight; and if he be a real dog, not a spoiled
pet, he will uncover more surprises on the familiar road than you
uncover in the morning newspaper. Follow him sympathetically as he
finds endlessly interesting things where you find little but boredom,
and you may learn that there are three marked differences between you
and a normal animal: first, that he keeps the spirit of play, which you
have well-nigh lost; second, that he lives in his sensations and is
happy, while you dwell mostly in your thoughts and postpone happiness
for the future; and third, that he is alive now, every moment, and you
used to be alive the day before yesterday.

In all this you are simply typical of our unnatural civilization, which
begins to enumerate or describe us as so many thousand “souls,”
doubtless because every single body of us is moribund or asleep.
Consider our noses, for example. They are the seat of a wonderful
faculty, more dependable than sight or hearing; they are capable of
giving us sensations more varied than those of color, and almost as
enjoyable as those of harmony; they can be easily trained so as to
recognize every tree and plant and growing thing by its delicate
fragrance; they would add greatly to our safety and convenience, as
well as to our enjoyment, did we use them as nature intended; yet so
thoroughly neglected are they, as a rule, that it takes a burning rag
or a jet of escaping coal-gas to rouse them to the immense and varied
world of odors in which an animal lives continually.

At present I am the alleged owner of a young setter, Rab, who has
reached the stage of development when he thinks he owns me. For after
you have properly trained a dog, there comes a brief time when he
discovers with joy that he can make you do things for him; and then he
is like a child who discovers that he can make you talk (that is, show
some sign of life) by asking you questions. And this young setter has,
I am convinced, a very low opinion of human aliveness, since there is
never a day when he does not give me a hint that he considers me a poor
cripple—excepting, perhaps, the rare days when I take him
woodcock-hunting, when he mildly approves of me.

One night, after waiting a long time at my feet till I should become
animate, Rab followed me hopefully into the dark kitchen, where he had
never before been allowed to go. I heard his steps behind me only as
far as the door, and thought he was afraid to enter; but when I turned
on the light, there he stood in the doorway, “frozen” into a beautiful
point, his head upturned to a bell and battery on the wall. Presently a
mouse hopped from the battery into a waste-basket on the floor, and Rab
pointed the thing stanchly, his whole body quivering with delight when
a faint odor stole to his nostrils or a rustle of paper to his ears.
From the basket the mouse streaked to the coal-hod, and Rab pointed
that, too, and then a crack under a door, and a yawning closet drawer.
“My bones!” he said, trying to claw the drawer open. “This is a good
place; something is alive here!” Then he came over and sat in front of
me, looking up in my face, his head twisted sideways, to demand why I
had so long kept him out of the only part of the house that was not
wholly dormant.

Though the adventure was twelve months ago, “age cannot wither it, nor
custom stale its infinite variety,” for every night Rab comes to me
where I am reading, nudges my elbow, paws my book, sits in front of me
to yawn hugely, gives me no peace, in short, till I follow him to the
kitchen for another go at the waste-basket, the coal-hod and all the
closet drawers. It is of no avail to leave the doors open; he will not
go unless I join him in the enjoyment of something that lives. A short
time since we flushed another mouse in the cellar, whither he followed
me to look after the furnace; and now he pesters me by day as well as
by night whenever he finds me, as he thinks, wrapped in unmanly
lethargy.

With all his vivacity this young setter, who seems so alert among mere
men, becomes a dull creature the moment you compare him with his wild
kindred. He cannot begin to interpret the world through his senses as
they instantly interpret it, and he would starve to death where they
live on the fat of the land. Once on a forest trail I came upon him
pointing stanchly at the edge of a little opening that lumbermen had
made for yarding logs. Just across the opening, where a jumper road
joined the yard, stood a noble buck, and he was “pointing,” too, for he
was face to face with such a creature as he had never before seen. Both
animals were like statues, so motionless did they stand; but there was
this difference, that the dog rested solidly on earth and might have
been carved from marble, while the buck seemed to rest on air and to be
compounded of some ethereal essence. His eyes fairly radiated light and
color. The velvet on his antlers seemed to grow as I looked upon it,
like the velvet moss in which the fairies are said to rest. Every hair
of him from nose to tail tip was gloriously alive. A moment only he
stood, but long enough for me to carry a picture of him forever
afterward; then he bounded up the old road, and Rab came running over
to ask me what new thing he had discovered.

This marvelous alertness of the natural animal is commonly attributed
to the fact that his physical senses are more acute than ours; but that
is true only of some particular sense of a certain creature. The wolf’s
nose, the deer’s ear, the vulture’s eye,—these are probably keener than
any similar human organ; but, on the other hand, a man’s eye is very
much keener for details than the eye of wolf or deer; his senses of
touch and taste are finer than anything to be found among the lower
orders, and the average of his five senses is probably the highest upon
earth. Yet the animal is more responsive to impressions of the external
world, and this is due, I think, to the fact that he lives more in his
sensations; that he is not cumbered, as we are, by inner phenomena;
that he is free from pain, care, fear, regret, anxiety and other mental
complexities; that he is accustomed to hear (that is, to receive
vibrations) through his feet or his skin quite as much as through his
ears; and that his whole body, relaxed and at ease, often becomes a
first-class receiving instrument for what we call sense impressions.

As an example of this last, note the wild creature’s response to every
change of air pressure, a response so immediate and certain that one
might accurately forestall the barometer by observing the action of
birds, or even of chickens, which anticipate a storm long before your
face has noted the moist wind or your eye the rain-cloud. In the winter
woods I have often seen a deer feeding greedily (quite at variance with
his usual dainty tasting) at an hour long before or long after his
accustomed time, and as I traveled wider I would find other deer doing
the same thing. I used to wonder at this, till I noticed that such
unusual action was always followed by a storm—not an ordinary brief
snowfall, to which deer pay little attention beyond seeking shelter
while it lasts, but a severe storm or blizzard, during which most
animals lie quiet for a whole day, or even two or three days, without
stirring abroad for food.

Whether an element of forethought enters into this act of “stuffing”
themselves before a storm, or whether it is wholly instinctive, like
the bear’s change of diet before he dens for his winter sleep, is a
question which does not concern us, since nobody can answer it. In
either case the deer have felt, as surely as our most sensitive
instruments, not only the decreasing pressure of the air but also its
increasing moisture.

That such sensitiveness is not of any one organ, but rather of the
whole body, becomes more evident when we study the lower orders—fishes,
for example, which may winter under a dozen fathoms of water and a
two-foot blanket of ice, but which nevertheless respond to the changing
air currents far above their heads. Once on a northern lake, in March,
I kept tabs on some trout for fourteen consecutive days, and it seemed
that they moved from deep to shallow water or back again whenever the
wind veered to the proper quarter. I had a water-hole cut in the thick
ice, and, finding a trout under it one day, I kept a couple of lines
with minnows there constantly. The hole was in a shallow place, over a
sandy bottom, and by putting my face to the opening, with a blanket
over my head to exclude the upper light, I could dimly see the shadows
move in from deep water. On six scattering days, when the wind came
light or strong from the south, two of the days bringing snow, the
trout evidently moved shoreward, since I caught them abundantly, as
many as I needed and some to spare; but on the other eight days, some
clear and some stormy, when the wind was north of east or west, not a
trout was seen or caught, and only once was a minnow of mine missing.

It may be said that the trout simply followed their food-supply; but I
doubt this, since trout apparently feed very little in winter; and in
formulating any theory of the matter one must account for the fact that
big fish or little fish moved shoreward whenever the wind blew south.
The phenomenon may appear less foreign to our experience, though not
less mysterious to our reason, if we remember that an old wound or a
corn may by its aching foretell a storm, or that a person suffering
from nervous prostration may by his sudden depression know that the
barometer will soon be falling.

The same bodily sensitiveness appears unchanged in our domestic
animals. I once saw a deer and her two fawns kneel down in the woods,
and watched them in astonishment as they rested for some time on their
knees, as if in supplication; then the ground rocked under me, and I
knew that their feet had felt the tremor of an earthquake long before I
was sensible of it. Such an observation seemed wonderful to me till I
learned that our sheep are equally sensitive in their elastic hoofs,
and that our pigs respond not only to vibration of earth or air, but
also to some finer vibration set in motion, apparently, by human
excitement.

Moreover, I have known one dog, old and half deaf, that, whether asleep
or awake, would respond to the faint tremor of his master’s automobile
before it came into sight or hearing. And what there is in the tremor
of one machine to distinguish it from another of the same make and
power is something that the unaided ear can hardly measure. The dog
lived under a hilltop, on a highway over which scores of automobiles
passed daily; and on holidays, when his master was at home, the scores
would increase to hundreds. He would sleep for hours on the veranda,
paying no heed to the noise or smell or dust of the outrageous things,
till suddenly he would jump up, bark, and start for the gate; and in a
moment or two we would see the master’s auto rise into sight over the
brow of the hill.



The instant response of deer or dog to minute external impressions,
though startling enough, is probably wholly physical, a matter of
vibrations on one side and of nerves on the other; but there are other
phenomena of sensitiveness (and these bring us nearer to our trail of
animal communication) for which it is much harder to find a
satisfactory physical explanation. Such is the feeling or warning of
unsensed danger, or the premonition that some one unseen and unheard is
approaching—a phenomenon which seems to be common among animals, to
judge from repeated observation, and which appears often enough in
human beings to make not only the inquisitive Society for Psychical
Research but almost every thoughtful man or woman take some note of it.

For example, a man awake in his bed sees his son, whom he thinks safely
ashore in a foreign country, fall overboard from a steamer to his
death, at the very hour when the son did fall overboard, as was
afterward learned. Or a woman, the wife of a sea-captain, sitting on
the veranda at home in the bright moonlight, sees the familiar earth
vanish in a world of water, and looks suddenly upon her husband’s ship
as it reels to the gale, turns over to the very edge of destruction,
and then rights itself with half its crew swept overboard—and all this
while the precise event befell a thousand miles away. Such things,
which spell a different kind of sensitiveness from that with which we
are familiar, have happened to people well known to me; but as they
have happened to others also, and as almost every town or village has a
convincing example of its own, I forbear details and accept the fact,
and try here to view or understand it as a natural phenomenon.

At first you may strongly object even to my premise, calling it
incredible that sense-bound mortals should feel a danger that their
eyes cannot see or their ears hear; but there are at least two
reasonable answers to your objection. In the first place, we are
sense-bound only in the sense of limiting ourselves unnecessarily,
confining our perception to five habitual modes, shamefully neglecting
to cultivate even these, and ignoring the use or the existence of other
and perhaps finer means of contact with the external world. Again, it
is not a whit more incredible that sensitive creatures, whether brute
or human, should feel the coming or going of a person than that they
should feel his look or glance, as they certainly do.

This last is no cloudy theory; it is a plain fact which endures the
test of observation. Almost any man of strong personality can disturb
or awaken a sleeping wild animal simply by looking at him intently; and
the nearer the man is the more certain the effect of his gaze on the
sleeping brute. The same is true in less degree of most Indians and
woodsmen, and of many sensitive women and children, as you may prove
for yourself. Go into a room where a sensitive or “high-strung” person
is taking a nap—not sleeping heavily, as most men sleep, but lightly,
naturally, as all wild animals take their rest. Make no noise, but
stand or sit quietly where you can look intently into the sleeper’s
face; and commonly, by a change of position or a turning away of the
head or a startled opening of the eyes, the sleeper will show that he
feels your look and is trying subconsciously to avoid it. The
awakening, whether of animals or of men, does not always follow our
look, most fortunately, but it happens frequently enough under varying
conditions to put the explanation of chance entirely out of the
question.

When I was a child I used to sit long hours in the woods alone, partly
for love of the breathing solitude, and partly for getting acquainted
with wild birds or beasts, which showed no fear of me when they found
me quiet. At such times I often found within myself an impression which
I expressed in the words, “Something is watching you.” Again and again,
when nothing stirred in my sight, that curious warning would come; and
almost invariably, on looking around, I would find some bird or fox or
squirrel which had probably caught a slight motion of my head and had
halted his roaming to creep near and watch me inquisitively. As I grew
older the “feel” of living things grew dimmer; yet many times in later
years, when I have been in the wilderness alone, I have experienced the
same impression of being watched or followed, and so often has it
proved a true warning that I still trust it and act upon it, even when
my eyes see nothing unusual and my ears hear nothing but their own
ringing in the silence.

I remember once, when I was sitting on the shore of a lake at twilight,
that I began to have an increasing impression that some living thing
unseen was near me. At first I neglected it, for I had my eyes on a
deer that interested me greatly; but the feeling grew stronger till I
obeyed it and rose to my feet. At the first motion came a startling
woof! and from some bushes close behind me a bear jumped away for the
woods. No doubt he had been there some time, watching me or creeping
nearer, knowing that I was alive, but completely puzzled by my
shapelessness and lack of action. A similar thing has happened several
times, in other places and with other animals, and always at a moment
when I was most in harmony with the environment and a sharer of its
deep tranquillity. [1]

As a child this faculty (if such it be) was as natural as anything else
in life; for in childhood we take the world as we find it in personal
experience, and nothing is especially wonderful where all is wonder. I
then thought no more of feeling the presence of an animal than of
hearing him walk when I could not see him; but as I grew older the
experience seemed a little odd or “queer,” and I never spoke of it to
any one till I discovered, first, that the faculty seems to be common
among animals, and second, that some Indians (not all) have it and
regard it as the most natural thing in the world. Here is how the
latter discovery came about:

Simmo and I were calling moose from a lake one moonlit night, with a
silent canoe under us, dark evergreen woods at our back, and a little
ghost of a beaver-meadow, vague, misty, shadow-filled, immediately
before our eyes yet seeming as remote as any drifting cloud. To our
repeated call no answer was returned; then we allowed the canoe to
drift ashore where it would, and sat listening to the vast silence. I
was brought back from my absorption in the fragrance, the harmony, the
infinite stillness of the night by feeling the canoe shake and hearing
Simmo whisper, “Somet’ing near. Look out!”

Now in a silence like that, the tense, living silence of the wilderness
at night, one’s ears are as full of tricks as a puckwudgie, who is one
of the mischievous fairy-folk of the Indians. The whine of a mosquito
sounds across the whole lake; or the mutter of a frog becomes like the
roar of a bull of Bashan; or suddenly you begin to hear music as the
faint vibration of some dry stub, purring in the unfelt air currents,
turns to the booming of a mighty church organ. At such a time, unless
one has trained his senses to ignore the obvious (the Indian secret of
seeing and hearing things), one soon becomes confused, uncertain of the
borderland between the real and the imaginary; so presently I turned to
Simmo and whispered, “You hear him?”

The Indian shook his head. “No hear-um; just feel-um,” he said; and
again we settled down to watch. For several minutes we questioned the
woods, the lake, the meadow; but nothing stirred, not a sound broke the
painful quiet, the while we both felt strongly that some living thing
was near us. Then a shadow moved from the darker shadow of an upturned
root, only a few yards away, and a great bull stood alert on the open
shore.

I thought then, and I still think, that besides our ordinary five
senses we have a finer faculty which I must call, for lack of a better
term, the sense of presence; and I explain it on the assumption that
every life recognizes and attracts every other life by some occult
force, as dead matter (if there be such a thing) attracts all other
matter by the mysterious force of gravitation.

Doubtless I have been many times watched in the woods when I did not
know or feel it; and certainly I have often had my eyes upon an
unconscious bird or beast when, though I fancied he grew uneasy under
my scrutiny, he did not have enough sense of danger to run
away—possibly because no real danger threatened him, the eyes that
looked upon him being friendly or merely curious.

Once on a hardwood ridge I came upon a buck lying asleep in open
timber, and stood with my back against a great sugar-maple, observing
him for five or six minutes before he stirred. No, I was not trying to
awaken him by a look, or to stage any other experiment. I was simply
enjoying a rare sight, noting with immense interest that this wild
creature, whom I had always seen so splendidly alert, could nod and
blink like any ordinary mortal. So near was he that I could mark the
drowsiness of his eyes, the position of his feet, the swelling of his
sides as he breathed. I happened to be counting his respirations in
friendly fashion, comparing them with my own, when suddenly his head
turned to me; his eyes snapped wide open, and they were looking
straight into mine. Apparently his feeling or subconscious perception
had warned him where danger was; but still his eyes could not recognize
it, standing there in plain sight. For to stand motionless without
concealment is often the best way to deceive a wild animal, which
habitually associates life with motion.

No more drowsiness for that buck! He was startled, plainly enough; but
he rose to his feet very stealthily, not stirring a leaf, and stood at
tense attention. When he turned his head to look over his shoulder
behind him I raised my field-glass, for I wanted to read his thought,
if possible, in his eyes. As he turned his gaze my way again his nose
seemed to sweep my face. It rested there a moment full on the lens of
my glass, moved on, and returned for a longer inspection. Then he
glided past, still without recognizing me, testing the air at every
springy step, harking this way, looking that way, and disappearing at
last as if he trod on eggs.

From such experiences I judge that the feeling of unsensed danger, or
the more subtle feel of a living thing, is as variable in the animals
as are their instincts or their social habits. It may be dull in one
creature and keen in another of the same species, or alternately awake
and asleep in the same creature; but there is no longer any doubt in my
mind that it is a widespread gift among birds and beasts. When it
appears occasionally among men, therefore, it is to be regarded not as
uncanny or queer, but, like the sure sense of direction which a few men
possess, as a precious and perfectly natural inheritance from our
primitive ancestors. That it skips a dozen generations to alight on an
odd man here or there, like a storm-driven bird on a ship at sea, is
precisely what we might expect of heredity, which follows a course that
seems to us erratic or at times marvelous, as geometry appears to an
Eskimo, because we do not yet understand its law or working principle.

Among savage tribes, who live a natural outdoor life in close contact
with nature, the perception of danger or of persons beyond the ordinary
sense range is much more common than among civilized folk. Almost every
explorer and missionary who has spent much time with African natives,
for example, has noticed that the Blacks have some mysterious means of
knowing when a stranger is approaching one of their
villages—mysterious, that is, because it does not depend on runners or
messengers or any other of our habitual means of communication. A
recent observer of these people has at last offered an explanation of
the matter, with many other impressions of native philosophy, in a work
to which he gives the suggestive title of Thinking Black. Of the scores
of books on Africa which I have read, this is the only one to show more
than a very superficial knowledge of the natives, and the reason is
apparent. The author thinks, most reasonably, that you cannot possibly
know the native or understand his customs until you know his thought,
and that the only trail to his thought is through the language in which
the thought is expressed. Therefore did he study the speech as a means
of knowing the man; and herein he is in refreshing contrast to other
African travelers and hunters I have read, who spend a few months or
weeks in a white man’s camp, knowing the natives about as intimately as
lovers know the moon, and then babble of native customs or beliefs or
“superstitions,”—as if any rite or habit could be understood without
first understanding the philosophy of life from which it sprang, as a
flower from a hidden seed.

This rare observer, who knows how the native thinks because he
perfectly understands the native language, tells us [2] that the Blacks
clearly recognize the power of animals and of normal men to know many
things beyond their sense range; that they give it a definite name, and
explain it in a way which indicates an astonishing degree of abstract
thought on the part of those whom we ignorantly call unthinking
savages. According to these natives, every natural animal, man
included, has the physical gifts of touch, sight, hearing, taste,
smell, and chumfo. I must still use the native term because it cannot
be translated, because it implies all that we mean by instinct,
intuitive or absolute knowledge and (a thing which no other psychology
has even hinted at) the process by which such knowledge is acquired.

This chumfo is not a sixth or extra sense, as we assume, but rather the
unity or perfect co-ordination of the five senses at their highest
point. I may illustrate the matter this way, still following Crawford,
whose record contains many curious bits of observation, savage
philosophy, woods lore and animal lore, many of them written by the
camp-fire and all jumbled together pell-mell:

For ordinary perception at near distances the eye or the ear is
sufficient, and while engaged in any near or obvious matter the five
senses work independently, each busy with its own function. But when
such observation is ended or at fault, and the man retreats, as it
were, into his inner self, then in the quiet all the senses merge and
harmonize into a single perfect instrument of perception. (Here, in
native dress, is nothing more or less than the psychologist’s
subconscious self, with its mysterious working.) At such moments the
whole animal or the whole man, not his brain and senses alone, becomes
sensitive to the most delicate impressions, to inaudible sounds or
vibrations, to unseen colors, to unsmelled odors or intangible
qualities,—to a multitude of subtle messages from the external world,
which are ordinarily unnoticed because the senses are ordinarily
separate, each occupied with its particular message. So when a sleeping
animal is suddenly aware that he must be alert, he does not learn of
approaching danger through his ears or nose, but through chumfo,
through the perfect co-ordination of all his senses working together as
one. In the same way a wandering black man always knows where his hut
or camp is; he holds his course on the darkest night, finds his way
through a vast jungle, goes back to any spot in it where he left
something, and often astonishes African travelers by getting wind of
their doings while they are yet far distant.

Such is the native philosophy; and the striking feature of it is, that
it is not superstitious but keenly observant, not ignorant but rational
and scientific, since it seems to anticipate our latest biological
discoveries, or rather, as we shall see, a philosophy which rests upon
biological science as a foundation.

Perhaps the first suggestion that the native may have reason in his
theory comes from the extraordinary sensitiveness of certain blind
people, who walk confidently about a cluttered room, or who sort the
family linen after it returns from the laundry. These blind people say,
and think, that they avoid objects by feeling the increasing air
pressure as they approach, or that they sort the family linen by smell;
but it appears more likely that a greater unity and refinement of all
their senses results from their living in darkness.

That our human senses have unused possibilities, or that we may
possibly possess extra senses of which we are not conscious, may appear
if you study the phenomenon of hearing, especially if you study it when
you hear a strange sound in the woods or in the house at night. It is
assumed that we always locate a sound by the ear, and that we determine
its volume or distance by our judgment from previous experiences; but
that, I think, is a secondary and not a primary process. When we act
most naturally we seem to locate a sound not by search or experiment,
but instantly, instinctively, absolutely; and then by our ear or our
judgment we strive to verify our first chumfo impression.

As a specific instance, you are lying half asleep at night when a
faint, strange sound breaks in upon your consciousness. If you act
naturally now, you will nine times out of ten locate the sound on the
instant, without bringing your “good ear” to bear upon it; but if you
neglect or lose your first impression, you may hunt for an hour and go
back to bed without finding the source of the disturbance. Or you are
traveling along a lonely lane at night, more alive than you commonly
are, when a sudden cry breaks out of the darkness. It lasts but a
fraction of a second and is gone; yet in that fleeting instant you have
learned three things: you know the direction of the cry; you know,
though you never heard it before, whether it is a loud cry from a
distance or a faint cry from near at hand; and you are so sure of its
exact location that you go to a certain spot, whether near or far, and
say, “That cry sounded here.”

So much if you act naturally; but if you depend on your ears or
judgment, as men are apt to do, then you are in for a long chase before
you locate the cry of a bird or a beast or a lost child in the night.

It is easy to make mistakes here, for we are so cumbered by artificial
habits that it is difficult to follow any purely natural process; but
our trail becomes clearer when we study the matter of hearing among the
brutes. Thus, your dog is lying asleep by the fire when a faint noise
or footstep sounds outside. Sometimes, indeed, there is no audible
sound at all when he springs to his feet; but no sooner is the door
opened for him than he is around the house and away, heading as
straight for the disturbance as if he knew, as he probably does,
exactly where to find it. Yet your dog is, I repeat, a very dull
creature in comparison with his wild kindred. Their ability to locate a
sound is almost unbelievable, not because they have more delicate ears
(for the human ear is much finer, being sensitive to a thousand
inflections, tones, harmonies, which are meaningless to the brute), but
because of what the Blacks call their better chumfo or what we
thoughtlessly call their stronger instincts.

This has been strongly impressed upon me at times when I have tried to
call a moose in the wilderness. If you seek these animals far back
where they are never hunted—a difficult matter nowadays—the bulls
answer readily enough, or sometimes too readily, as when one big brute
chased me to my canoe and gave me a hatless run for it; but in a
much-hunted region they are very shy and come warily to a call. The
best way to see them in such a place is to call a few times at night,
or until you get an answer, and then go quickly away before the bull
comes near enough to begin circling suspiciously. At daybreak you are
very apt to find him waiting; and the astonishing thing is that he is
waiting at the very spot where you used your trumpet.

The place you select for calling may be a tiny bog in a vast forest, or
a little, nameless beaver-meadow by a lake or river. It is like many
other such places, near or far, and the bull may come from a distance,
crossing lakes, rivers, bogs and dense forest on his way; but he never
seems to make a mistake or to be at a loss in locating the call. On a
still night I have heard a bull answer me from a mountain five or six
miles away; yet in the morning there he was, waiting expectantly for
his mate near the bit of open shore where I had called him; and to
reach that spot he must either have crossed the lake by swimming, a
distance of two miles, or else have circled it on a wide détour. That
he should come such a distance through woods and waters, and pick the
right spot from a hundred others on either side, seems to me not a
matter of ears or experience but of chumfo, or absolute knowledge.

Another and more interesting verification of the chumfo philosophy is
open to any man who will go quietly through the big woods by moonlight,
putting himself back amid primal or animal conditions, and observing
himself closely as he does so. The man who has not traveled the
wilderness alone at night has a vivid and illuminating experience
awaiting him. He is amazed, so soon as he overcomes the first unnatural
feeling of fear, to find how alive he is, and how much better he can
hear and smell than ever he dreamed. At such a time one’s whole body
seems to become a delicately poised instrument for receiving sense
impressions, and one’s skin especially begins to tingle and creep as it
wakes from its long sleep. Nor is this “creeping” of the skin strange
or queer, as we assume, but perfectly natural. The sensations which we
now ignorantly associate with fear of the dark (a late and purely human
development; the animal knows it not) are in reality the sensations of
awakening life.

Possibly we may explain this supersensitiveness of the skin, when life
awakens in it once more and it becomes for us another and finer
instrument of perception, by the simple biological fact that every cell
of the multitudes which make up the human body has a more or less
complete organization within itself. Moreover, as late experiments have
shown, a cell or a tissue of cells will live and prosper in a suitable
environment when completely separated from the body of which it was
once a part. These human cells inherit certain characteristics common
to all animal cells since life began; and it is not improbable that
they inherit also something of the primal cell’s sensibility, or
capacity to receive impressions from the external world. This universal
cell-function was largely given over when the animal (a collection of
cells) began to develop special organs of touch, sight, and hearing;
but there is no indication that the original power of sensibility has
ever been wholly destroyed in any cell. It is, therefore, still within
the range of biological possibility that a man should hear with his
fingers or smell with his toes, since every cell of both finger and toe
once did a work corresponding to the present functions of the five
animal senses.

Before you dismiss this as an idle or impossible theory, try a simple
experiment, which may open your eyes to the reality of living things.
Go to a greenhouse and select a spot of bare earth under a growing
rose-bush. Examine the surface carefully, then brush and examine it
again, to be sure that not a root of any kind is present. Now place a
handful of good plant-food on the selected spot, and go away to your
own affairs. Return in a week or so, brush aside your “bait,” and there
before your eyes is a mass of white feeding-roots where no root was
before. In some way, deep under the soil, the hungry cells have heard
or smelled or felt a rumor of food, and have headed for it as surely as
a dog follows his nose to his dinner.

Do plants, then, know what they do when they turn to the light, and is
there something like consciousness in a tree or a blade of grass? That
is too much to assert, though one may think or believe so. No man can
answer the question which occurs so constantly to one who lives among
growing things; but you can hardly leave your simple experiment without
formulating a theory that even the hidden rootlets of a rose-bush have
something fundamentally akin to our highly developed sensibility. At
present some biologists are beginning to assert, and confidently,
though it is but an opinion, that there is no dead matter in the world;
that the ultimate particles of which matter is composed are all
intensely alive. And if alive, they must be sentient; that is, each
must have an infinitesimal degree of feeling or sense perception.



To return from our speculation, and to illustrate the chumfo faculty
from human and animal experience: I was once sitting idly on a
Nantucket wharf, alternately watching some hermit-crabs scurrying about
in their erratic fashion under the tide, and an old dog that lay
soaking himself in the warm sunshine. Just behind us, the only
inharmonious creatures in the peaceful scene, some laborers were
unloading rocks from a barge by the aid of a derrick. For more than an
hour, or ever since I came to the wharf, the dog lay in the same spot,
and in all that time I did not see him move a muscle. He was apparently
sound asleep. Suddenly he heaved up on his rheumatic legs, sniffed the
air alertly, and turned his head this way, that way, as if wary of
something.

The human labor had proceeded lazily, for the day was warm; there was
no change in the environment, so far as I could discern; the only
sounds in the air were the sleepy lap of wavelets and the creaking of
pulleys; yet my instant thought was, “That dog is frightened; but at
what?” After a few moments of watching he moved off a dozen yards and
threw himself flat on his side to sleep again. His body was hardly
relaxed when a guy-rope parted, and the iron-bound mast of the derrick
crashed down on the wharf.

It was certainly “touch and go” for me; I felt the wind of the thing as
it fell, and was almost knocked off the wharf; but I was not thinking
then of my own close call. With my interest at high pitch I examined
the mast, and found it lying squarely athwart the impression left by
the dog in the dust of the road.

“Merely a coincidence,” you say; which indicates that we are apt to
think alike and in set formulas. That is precisely what I said at the
time—a mere coincidence, but a startling one, which made me think of
luck (a most foolish notion) and wonder why luck should elect to light
on a worthless old dog and take no heed of what seemed to me then a
precious young man. But I have since changed my mind; and here is one
of the many observations which made me change it.

Years afterward my Indian guide, Simmo, was camped with a white man
beside a salmon river. It was a rough night, and a storm was roaring
over the big woods. For shelter they had built a bark commoosie, and
for comfort a fire of birch logs. At about nine o’clock they turned in,
each wrapped in his blanket, and slept soundly but lightly, as woodsmen
do, after a long day on the trail. Some time later—hours, probably, for
the fire was low, the storm hushed, the world intensely still—the white
man was awakened by a touch, and opened his eyes to find his companion
in a tense, listening attitude.

“Bes’ get out of here quick, ’fore somet’ing come,” said the Indian,
and threw off his blanket.

“But why—what—how do you know?” queried the white man, startled but
doubting, for he had listened and heard nothing.

The Indian, angered as an Indian of the woods always is when you
question or challenge his craft, made an impatient gesture. “Don’ know
how; don’ know why; just know. Come!” he called sharply, and the white
man followed him away from the camp toward the river, where it was
lighter. For several minutes they stood there, like two alert animals,
searching the dark woods with all their senses; but nothing moved. The
white man was beginning another fool question when there came a sudden
dull crack, a booming of air, as a huge yellow-birch stub toppled over
the fire and flattened the commoosie like a bubble.

“Dere! Das de feller mus’ be comin’,” said Simmo. “By cosh, now, nex’
time Injun tell you one t’ing, p’r’aps you believe-um!” And, as if it
were the most natural thing in the world, he stepped over
death-and-destruction, kicked aside some rubbish, and lay down to sleep
where he was before.

“How do I explain it?” I don’t. I simply recognize a fact which I
cannot explain, and which I will not blink by calling it another
coincidence. For the fact is, as I judge, that a few men and many
animals exercise some extra faculty which I do not or cannot exercise,
or have access to some source of information which is closed to me.
When I question the gifted men or women who possess this faculty, or
what’s-its-name, I find that they are as much in the dark about it as I
am. They know certain things without knowing how they learn; and the
only word of explanation they offer is that they “feel” thus and
so—perhaps as a horse feels when he is holding the right direction
through a blinding snowstorm, as he does hold it, steadily, surely, if
you are wise enough not to bother him with the reins or your opinions.

Simmo is one of these rare men. At one moment he is a mere child, so
guileless, so natural, so innocent of worldly wisdom, that he is
forever surprising you. Once when his pipe was lost I saw him fill an
imaginary bowl, scratch an imaginary match, and puff away with a look
of heavenly content on his weathered face. So you treat him as an
unspoiled creature, humoring him, till there is difficulty or danger
ahead, or a man’s work to be done, when he steps quietly to the front
as if he belonged there. Or you may be talking with him by the
camp-fire, elaborating some wise theory, when he brushes aside your
book knowledge as of no consequence and suddenly becomes a philosopher,
proclaiming a new or startling doctrine of life in the sublimely
unhampered way of Emerson, who finished off objectors by saying, “I do
not argue; I know.” But where Emerson gives you a mystical word or a
bare assertion which he cannot possibly prove, Simmo has a
disconcerting way of establishing a challenged doctrine by a concrete
and undeniable fact.

One misty day when we were astray in the wilderness, he and I, we
attempted to travel by getting our compass bearings from the topmost
twigs of the evergreens, which slant mostly in one direction. After
blundering around for a time without getting any nearer camp or
familiar landmarks, Simmo remarked: “Dese twigs lie like devil. I guess
I bes’ find-um way myself.” And he did find it, and hold it even after
darkness fell, by instinctive feeling. At least, I judge it to have
been a matter of feeling rather than of sense or observation, for his
only explanation was, “Oh, w’en I goin’ right I feel good; but w’en I
goin’ wrong I oneasy.”

This natural feeling or impression of things beyond the range of sight,
this extra sense, or chumfo unity of all the senses, is probably akin
to another feeling by which the animal or man becomes aware of distant
persons, or of distant moods or emotions. The sleeping dog’s alarm
beneath the weakened derrick, or the sleeping Indian’s uneasiness near
the doomed birch-stub, might be explained on purely physical grounds:
some tremor of parting fibers, some warning vibration too faint for
eardrums but heavy enough to shake a more delicately poised nerve
center, reached the inner beast or the inner man and roused him to
impending danger. (There is a deal of babble in this explanation, I
admit, and still a mystery at the end of it.) But when a man or a brute
receives knowledge not of matter, but of minds or spirits like his own;
when a mother knows, for example, the mental state of a son who is far
away, and when no material vibrations of any known medium can pass
between them,—then all sixth-sense theories, which must rest on the
impinging of waves upon nerve centers, no longer satisfy or explain. We
are in the more shadowy region of thought transference or impulse
transference, and it is in this silent, unexplored region that, as I
now believe, a large part of animal communication goes on continually.

That belief will grow more clear, and perhaps more reasonable, if you
follow this unblazed trail a little farther.








IV

NATURAL TELEPATHY


The way of animal communication now grows dimmer and dimmer, or some
readers may even think it “curiouser and curiouser,” as Alice of
Wonderland said when she found herself lengthening out like a
telescope. But there is certainly a trail of some kind ahead, and since
we are apt to lose it or to wander apart, let us agree, if we can, upon
some familiar fact or experience which may serve as a guiding landmark.
Our general course will be as follows: first, to define our subject, or
rather, to make its meaning clear by illustration; second, to examine
the reasonableness of telepathy from a natural or biological viewpoint;
and finally, to go afield with eyes and minds open to see what the
birds or the beasts may teach us of this interesting matter.

It seems to be fairly well established that a few men and women of
uncommonly fine nervous organization (which means an uncommonly natural
or healthy organization) have the power of influencing the mind of
another person at a distance; and this rare power goes by the name of
thought transference, or telepathy. The so-called crossing of letters,
when two widely separated persons sit down at the same hour to write
each other on the same subject, is the most familiar but not the most
convincing example of the thing. Yes, I know the power and the example
are both challenged, since there are scientists who deny telepathy root
and branch, as well as scientists who believe in it implicitly; but I
also know something more convincing than any second-hand denial or
belief, having at different times met three persons who used the “gift”
so freely, and for the most part so surely, that to ignore it would be
to abandon confidence in my own sense and judgment. I am not trying,
therefore, to investigate an opinion, but to understand a fact.

To illustrate the matter by a personal experience: For many years after
I first left home my mother would become “uneasy in her mind,” as she
expressed it, whenever a slight accident or danger or sickness had
befallen me. If the event were to me serious or threatening, there was
no more doubt or uneasiness on my mother’s part. She would know within
the hour that I was in trouble of some kind, and would write or
telegraph to ask what was the matter.

It is commonly assumed that any such power must be a little weird or
uncanny; that it contradicts the wholesome experience of humanity or
makes fantastic addition to its natural faculties; and I confess that
the general queerness, the lack of balance, the Hottentotish credulity
of folk who dabble in occult matters give some human, if not
reasonable, grounds for the assumption. Nevertheless, I judge that
telepathy is of itself wholly natural; that it is a survival, an
age-old inheritance rather than a new invention or discovery; that it
might be exercised not by a few astonishing individuals, but by any
normal man or woman who should from infancy cultivate certain mental
powers which we now habitually neglect. I am led to this conviction
because I have found something that very much resembles telepathy in
frequent use throughout the entire animal kingdom. It is, as I think
and shall try to make clear, a natural gift or faculty of the animal
mind, which is largely subconscious, and it is from the animal mind
that we inherit it; just as a few woodsmen inherit the animal sense of
direction, and cultivate and trust it till they are sure of their way
in any wilderness, while the large majority of men, dulled by
artificial habit, go promptly astray whenever they venture beyond
beaten trails.

That the animals inherit this power of silent communication over great
distances is occasionally manifest even among our half-natural domestic
creatures. For example, that same old setter of mine, Don, who
introduced us to our fascinating subject, was left behind most
unwillingly during my terms at school; but he always seemed to know
when I was on my way home. For months at a stretch he would stay about
the house, obeying my mother perfectly, though she never liked a dog;
but on the day I was expected he would leave the premises, paying no
heed to orders, and go to a commanding ledge beside the lane, where he
could overlook the highroad. Whatever the hour of my coming, whether
noon or midnight, there I would find him waiting.

Once when I was homeward bound unexpectedly, having sent no word of my
coming, my mother missed Don and called him in vain. Some hours later,
when he did not return at his dinner-time or answer her repeated call,
she searched for him and found him camped expectantly in the lane.
“Oho! wise dog,” said she. “I understand now. Your master is coming
home.” And without a doubt that it would soon be needed, she went and
made my room ready.

If the dog had been accustomed to spend his loafing-time in the lane,
one might thoughtlessly account for his action by the accident or
hit-or-miss theory; but he was never seen to wait there for any length
of time except on the days when I was expected. And once (unhappily the
last time Don ever came to meet his master) he was observed to take up
his watch within a few minutes of the hour when my train left the
distant town. Apparently he knew when I headed homeward, but there was
nothing in his instinct or experience to tell him how long the journey
might be. So he would wait patiently, loyally, knowing I was coming,
and my mother would take his dinner out to him.

In many other ways Don gave the impression, if not the evidence, that
he was a “mind-reader.” He always knew when Saturday came, or a
holiday, and possibly he may have associated the holiday notion with my
old clothes; but how he knew what luck the day had in store for him, as
he often seemed to know the instant I unsnapped his chain in the early
morning, was a matter that at first greatly puzzled me. If I appeared
in my old clothes and set him free with the resolution that my day must
be spent in study or tinkering or farm work, he would bid me good
morning and go off soberly to explore the premises, as dogs are wont to
do. But when I met him silently with the notion that the day was my day
off, to be wasted in shooting or fishing or roving the countryside,
then in some way Don caught the notion instantly; he would be tugging
at his leash before I reached him, and no sooner was he free than he
was all over the yard in mad capers or making lunatic attempts to drag
me off on our common holiday before breakfast.

That any dog of mine should obey my word, doing gladly whatever I told
him, was to be expected; or that in the field he should watch for a
motion of my hand and follow it instantly, whether to charge or hold or
come in or cast left or right, was a simple matter of training; but
that this particular dog should, unknown to me, enter into my very
feeling, was certainly not the result of education, and probably not of
sight or sense, as we ordinarily understand the terms. When we were
together of an evening before the fire, so long as I was working or
pleasantly reading he would lie curled up on his own mat, without ever
disturbing me till it was time for him to be put to bed, when he would
remind me of the fact by nudging my elbow. But if an hour came when I
was in perplexity, or had heard bad news and was brooding over it,
hardly would I be away in thought, forgetful of Don’s existence on a
trail I must follow alone, when his silky head would slide under my
hand, and I would find his brown eyes searching my face with something
inexpressibly fine and loyal and wistful in their questioning deeps.

Thus repeatedly, unexpectedly, Don seemed to enter into my moods by
some subtle, mysterious perception for which I have no name, and no
explanation save the obvious one—that a man’s will or emotion may fill
a room with waves or vibration as real as those streaming from a fire
or a lighted candle, and that normal animals have some unused bodily
faculty for receiving precisely such messages or vibrations. But we are
not yet quite ready for that part of our trail; it will come later,
when we can follow it with more understanding.

Should this record seem to you too personal (I am dealing only with
first-hand impressions of animal life), here is the story of another
dog—not a blue-blooded or highly trained setter, but just an ordinary,
doggy, neglected kind of dog—submitted by a scientific friend of mine,
who very cautiously offers no explanation, but is content to observe
and verify the facts:

This second dog, Watch by name and nature, was accustomed to meet his
master much as Don met me in the lane; but he did it much more
frequently, and timed the meeting more accurately. He was nearer the
natural animal, never having been trained in any way, and perhaps for
that reason he retained more of the natural gift or faculty of
receiving a message from a distance. His owner, a busy carpenter and
builder, had an office in town, and was accustomed to return from his
office or work at all hours, sometimes early in the afternoon, and
again long after dark. At whatever hour the man turned homeward, Watch
seemed to follow his movement as if by sight; he would grow uneasy,
would bark to be let out if he happened to be in the house, and would
trot off to meet his master about half-way. Though he was occasionally
at fault, and sometimes returned to brood over the matter when his
master, having started for home, was turned aside by some errand, his
mistakes were decidedly exceptional rather than typical. His strange
“gift” was a matter of common knowledge in the neighborhood, and
occasionally a doubtful man would stage an experiment: the master would
agree to mark the hour when he turned homeward, and one or more
interested persons would keep tabs on the dog. So my scientific friend
repeatedly tested Watch, and observed him to take the road within a few
moments of the time when his master left his office or building
operations in the town, some three or four miles away.

Thus far the record is clear and straight, but there is one important
matter which my friend overlooked, as scientific men commonly do when
they deal with nature, their mistake being to regard animals as
featureless members of a class or species rather than as individuals.
The dog’s master always came or went in a wagon drawn by a quiet old
horse, and upon inquiry I found that between Watch and the horse was a
bond of comradeship, such as often exists between two domestic animals
of different species. Thus, the dog often preferred to sleep in the
stall near his big chum, or would accompany him to the pasture when he
was turned loose, and would always stand by, as if overlooking the
operation, when the horse was being harnessed. It may well be,
therefore, that it was from the horse rather than from the man that
Watch received notice when heads were turned homeward; but of the fact
that some kind of telepathic communication passed between two members
of the trio there is no reasonable doubt.

Some of my readers may make objection at this point that, though
something like telepathic communication appears now and then among the
brutes, it should be regarded as merely freakish or sensational, like a
two-headed calf; while others will surely ask, “Why, if our dogs
possess such a convenient faculty, do they not use it more frequently,
more obviously, and so spare themselves manifold discomforts or
misunderstandings?”

Such an objection is natural enough, since we judge as we live, mostly
by habit; but it has no validity, I think, and for two reasons. First,
because such animals as we have thus far seen exercising the faculty
(and they are but a few out of many) are apparently normal and sensible
beasts, precisely like their less-gifted fellows; and second, because
the telepathic power itself, when one examines it without prejudice,
appears to be wholly natural, and sane or simple as the power of
thought, even of such rudimentary thought as may be exercised in an
animal’s head. As for emotions, more intense and penetrating than any
thought, it is hardly to be questioned that a man’s fear or panic may
flow through his knees into the horse he is riding, or that emotional
excitement may spread through a crowd of men without visible or audible
expression. That a dog should receive a wordless message or impulse
from his master at a distance of three or four miles is, fundamentally,
no more unnatural than that one man should feel another’s mood at a
distance of three or four feet. Whether we can explain the phenomenon
on strictly biological or scientific grounds is another matter.

I am not a biologist, unfortunately, and must go cat-footedly when I
enter that strange garret. I look with wonder on these patient,
unemotional men who care nothing for a bear or an eagle, but who creep
lower and ever lower in the scale of living things, searching with
penetrating looks among infinitesimal microbes for the secret that
shall solve the riddle of the universe by telling us what life is. And
because man is everywhere the same, watching these exploring biologists
I remember the curious theology of certain South-Pacific savages, who
say that God made all things, the stars and the world and the living
man; but we cannot see Him because He is so very small, because a
dancing mote or a grain of sand is for Him a roomy palace. Yet even
with a modest little knowledge of biology we may find a viewpoint, I
think, from which telepathy or thought-transference would appear as
natural, as inevitable, as the forthgoing of light from a burning lamp.

Thus, historically there was a time when the living cell, or the
cell-of-life, as one biologist calls it with rare distinction, was
sensitive only to pressure; when in its darkness it knew of an external
world only by its own tremblings, in response to vibrations which
poured over it from every side. Something made it tremble, and that
“something” had motion or life like its own. Such, imaginatively, was
the sentient cell’s first knowledge, the result of a sense of touch
distributed throughout its protecting surface.

Long afterward came a time when the living cell, multiplied now a
millionfold, began to develop special sense-organs, each a modification
of its rudimentary sense of touch; one to receive vibrations of air,
for hearing; another to catch some of the thronging ether waves, for
seeing; a third to register the floating particles of matter on a
sensitive membrane, for taste or smelling. By that time the cell had
learned beyond a peradventure that the universe outside itself had
light and color and fragrance and harmony. Finally came a day when the
cell, still multiplying and growing ever more complex, became conscious
of a new power within itself, most marvelous of all the powers of
earth, the power to think, to feel, and to be aware of a self that
registered its own impressions of the external world. And then the cell
knew, as surely as it knew sound or light, that the universe held
consciousness also, and some infinite source of thought and feeling.
Such, apparently, was the age-long process from the sentient cell to
the living man.

Since we are following a different trail, this is hardly the time or
place to face the question how this development from mere living to
conscious life took place, even if one were wise or rash enough to
grapple with the final problem of evolution. Yet it may not be amiss
while we “rest a pipe,” as the voyageurs say, to point out that, of the
two possible answers to our question (aside from the convenient and
restful answer that God made things so), only one, curiously enough,
has thus far been considered by our physical scientists. The thousand
books and theories of evolution which one reads are all reducible to
this elementary proposition: that the simple things of life became
complex by inner necessity. In other words, an eye became an eye, or an
oak an oak, or a man a man, simply because each must develop according
to the inner law of its being.

That may be true, though the all-compelling “inner law” is still only a
vague assumption, and the mystery of its origin is untouched; but why
not by outer compulsion as reasonably as by inner necessity? A
cell-of-life that was constantly bombarded by moving particles of
matter might be compelled to develop a sense of touch, in order to save
its precious life by differentiating such particles into good and bad,
or helpful and harmful. A cell over which vibrations of air and ether
were continually passing might be forced for its own good to develop an
ear and an eye to receive such vibrations as sound and light; and a
cell over which mysterious waves of thought and emotion were
ceaselessly flowing might be driven to comprehend that particular
mystery by developing a thought and emotion of its own.

I do not say that this is the right answer; I mention it merely as a
speculative possibility, in order to get our alleged scientific mind
out of its deep rut of habit by showing that every road has two sides,
though a man habitually use only one; and that Reason or Law or God, or
whatever you choose to call the ultimate mainspring of life, is quite
as apt to be found on one side of the road as on the other. Inner
necessity is not a whit more logical or more explanatory than external
force or compulsion when we face the simple fact that an animal now
sees and feels in the light instead of merely existing in darkness, or
that primitive cells which were dimly sentient have now become as
thinking gods, knowing good and evil.

What this thought of ours is we do not know. Beyond the fact that we
have it and use it, thought still remains a profound mystery. That it
is a living force of some kind; that it projects itself or its waves
outward, as the sun cannot but send forth his light; that it affects
men as surely as gravitation or heat or the blow of a hammer affects
them,—all this is reasonably clear and certain. But how thought
travels; what refined mental ether conveys it outward with a speed that
makes light as slow as a glacier by comparison, and with a force that
sends it through walls of stone and into every darkness that the light
cannot penetrate,—this and the origin of thought are questions so deep
that our science has barely formulated them, much less dreamed of an
answer. Yet if we once grant the simple proposition that thought is a
force, that it moves inevitably from its source to its object, the
conclusion is inevitable that any thinking mind should be able to send
its silent message to any other mind in the universe. There is nothing
in the nature of either mind or matter to preclude such a possibility;
only our present habit of speech, of too much speech, prevents us from
viewing it frankly.

As a purely speculative consummation, therefore, the time may come when
telepathy shall appear as the natural or perfect communication among
enlightened minds, and language as a temporary or evolutionary
makeshift. But that beckons us away to an imaginative flight among the
clouds, and on the earth at our feet is the trail we must follow.

The question why our dogs, if they have the faculty of receiving a
master’s message at a distance, do not use it more obviously, is one
that I cannot answer. Perhaps the reason is obvious enough to some of
the dogs, which have a sidelong way of coming home from their roving,
as if aware they had long been wanted. Or, possibly, the difficulty
lies not in the dog, but in his master. Every communication has two
ends, one sending, the other receiving; and of a thousand owners there
are hardly two who know how properly to handle a dog either by speech
or by silence. Still again, one assumption implied in the question is
that dogs or any other animals of the same kind are all alike; and that
common assumption is very wide of the fact. Animals differ as widely in
their instinctive faculties as men in their judgments; which partly
explains why one setter readily follows his master’s word or hand, or
enters into his mood, while another remains hopelessly dumb or
unresponsive. The telepathic faculty appears more frequently, as we
shall see, among birds or animals that habitually live in flocks or
herds, and I have always witnessed its most striking or impressive
manifestation between a mother animal and her young, as if some
prenatal influence or control were still at work.

For example, I have occasionally had the good luck to observe a
she-wolf leading her pack across the white expanse of a frozen lake in
winter; and at such times the cubs have a doggish impulse to run after
any moving object that attracts their attention. If a youngster breaks
away to rush an animal that he sees moving in the woods (once that
moving animal was myself), the mother heads him instantly if he is
close to her; but if he is off before she can check him by a motion of
her ears or a low growl, she never wastes time or strength in chasing
him. She simply holds quiet, lifts her head high, and looks steadily at
the running cub. Suddenly he wavers, halts, and then, as if the look
recalled him, whirls and speeds back to the pack. If the moving object
be proper game afoot, the mother now goes ahead to stalk or drive it,
while the pack follows stealthily behind her on either side; but if the
distant object be a moose or a man, or anything else that a wolf must
not meddle with, then the mother wolf trots quietly on her way without
a sound, and the errant cub falls into place as if he had understood
her silent command.

You may observe the same phenomenon of silent order and ready obedience
nearer home, if you have patience to watch day after day at a burrow of
young foxes. I have spent hours by different dens, and have repeatedly
witnessed what seemed to be excellent discipline; but I have never yet
heard a vixen utter a growl or cry or warning of any kind. That audible
communication comes later, when the cubs begin to hunt for themselves;
and then you will often hear the mother’s querulous squall or the cubs’
impatient crying when they are separated in the dark woods. While the
den is their home (they seldom enter it after they once roam abroad)
silence is the rule, and that silence is most eloquent. For hours at a
stretch the cubs romp lustily in the afternoon sunshine, some stalking
imaginary mice or grasshoppers, others challenging their mates to mock
fights or mock hunting; and the most striking feature of the exercise,
after you have become familiar with the fascinating little creatures,
is that the old vixen, who lies apart where she can overlook the play
and the neighborhood, seems to have the family under perfect control at
every instant, though never a word is uttered.

That some kind of communication passes among these intelligent little
brutes is constantly evident; but it is without voice or language. Now
and then, when a cub’s capers lead him too far from the den, the vixen
lifts her head to look at him intently; and somehow that look has the
same effect as the she-wolf’s silent call; it stops the cub as if she
had sent a cry or a messenger after him. If that happened once, you
might overlook it as a matter of mere chance; but it happens again and
again, and always in the same challenging way. The eager cub suddenly
checks himself, turns as if he had heard a command, catches the vixen’s
look, and back he comes like a trained dog to the whistle.

As the shadows lengthen on the hillside, and the evening comes when the
mother must go mousing in the distant meadow, she rises quietly to her
feet. Instantly the play stops; the cubs gather close, their heads all
upturned to the greater head that bends to them, and there they stand
in mute intentness, as if the mother were speaking and the cubs
listening. For a brief interval that tense scene endures, exquisitely
impressive, while you strain your senses to catch its meaning. There is
no sound, no warning of any kind that ears can hear. Then the cubs
scamper quickly into the burrow; the mother, without once looking back,
slips away into the shadowy twilight. At the den’s mouth a foxy little
face appears, its nostrils twitching, its eyes following a moving
shadow in the distance. When the shadow is swallowed up in the dusk the
face draws back, and the wild hillside is wholly silent and deserted.

You can go home now. The vixen may be hours on her hunting, but not a
cub will again show his nose until she returns and calls him. If a
human mother could exercise such silent, perfect discipline, or leave
the house with the certainty that four or five lively youngsters would
keep out of danger or mischief as completely as young fox cubs keep out
of it, raising children might more resemble “one grand sweet song” than
it does at present.



So far as I have observed grown birds or beasts, the faculty of silent
communication occurs most commonly among those that are gregarious or
strongly social in their habits. The timber-wolves of the North are the
first examples that occur to me, and also the most puzzling. They are
wary brutes, so much so that those who have spent a lifetime near them
will tell you that it is useless to hunt a wolf by any ordinary method;
that your meeting with him is a matter of chance or rare accident; that
not only has he marvelously keen ears, eyes that see in the dark, and a
nose that cannot be deceived, but he can also “feel” a danger which is
hidden from sight or smell or hearing. Such is the Indian verdict; and
I have followed wolves often and vainly enough to have some sympathy
with it.

The cunning of these animals would be uncanny if it were merely
cunning; but it is naturally explained, I think, on the assumption that
wolves, more than most other brutes, receive silent warnings from one
another, or even from a concealed hunter, who may by his excitement
send forth some kind of emotional alarm. When you are sitting quietly
in the woods, and a pack of wolves pass near without noticing their one
enemy, though he is in plain sight, you think that they are no more
cunning than a bear or a buck; and that is true, so far as their
cunning depends on what they may see or hear. Once when I was crossing
a frozen lake in a snow-storm a whole pack of wolves rushed out of the
nearest cover and came at me on the jump, mistaking me for a deer or
some other game animal; which does not speak very highly for either
their eyes or their judgment. They were the most surprised brutes in
all Canada when they discovered their mistake. But when you hide with
ready rifle near some venison which the same wolves have killed; when
you see them break out of the woods upon the ice, running free and
confident to the food which they know is awaiting them; when you see
them stop suddenly, as if struck, though they cannot possibly see or
smell you, and then scatter and run by separate trails to a
meeting-point on another lake—well, then you may conclude, as I do,
that part of a wolf’s cunning lies deeper than his five senses.

Another lupine trait which first surprised and then challenged my
woodcraft is this: in the winter-time, when timber-wolves commonly run
in small packs, a solitary or separated wolf always seems to know where
his mates are hunting or idly roving or resting in their day-bed. The
pack is made up of his family relatives, younger or older, all mothered
by the same she-wolf; and by some bond or attraction or silent
communication he can go straight to them at any hour of the day or
night, though he may not have seen them for a week, and they have
wandered over countless miles of wilderness in the interim.

We may explain this fact, if such it be (I shall make it clear
presently), on the simple ground that the wolves, though incurable
rovers, have bounds beyond which they seldom pass; that they return on
their course with more or less regularity; and that in traveling, as
distinct from hunting, they always follow definite runways, like the
foxes. Because of these fixed habits, a solitary wolf might remember
that the pack was due in a certain region on a certain day, and by
going to that region and putting his nose to the runways he could
quickly pick up the fresh trail of his fellows. There is nothing occult
in such a process; it is a plain matter of brain and nose.

Such an explanation sounds reasonable enough; too reasonable, in fact,
since a brute probably acts more intuitively and less rationally; but
it does not account for the amazing certainty of a wounded wolf when
separated from his pack. He always does separate, by the way; not
because the others would eat him, for that is not wolf nature, but
because every stricken bird or beast seeks instinctively to be alone
and quiet while his hurt is healing. I have followed with keen interest
the doings of one wounded wolf that hid for at least two days and
nights in a sheltered den, after which he rose from his bed and went
straight as a bee’s flight to where his pack had killed a buck and left
plenty of venison behind them.

In this case it is possible to limit the time of the wounded wolf’s
seclusion, because the limping track that led from the den was but a
few hours old when I found it, and the only track leading into the den
was half obliterated by snow which had fallen two nights previously.
How many devious miles the pack had traveled in the interim would be
hard to estimate. I crossed their hunting or roaming trails at widely
separate points, and once I surprised them in their day-bed; but I
never found the limit of their great range. A few days later that same
limping wolf left another den of his, under a windfall, and headed not
for the buck, which was now frozen stiff, but for another deer which
the same pack had killed in a different region, some eight or ten
straight miles away, and perhaps twice that distance as wolves commonly
travel.

If you contend that this wounded wolf must have known where the meat
was by the howling of the pack when they killed, I grant that may be
true in one case, but certainly not in the other. For by great good
luck I was near the pack, following a fresh trail in the gray,
breathless dawn, when the wolves killed the second deer; and there was
not a sound for mortal ears to hear, not a howl or a trail cry or even
a growl of any kind. They followed, killed and ate in silence, as
wolves commonly do, their howling being a thing apart from their
hunting. The wounded wolf was then far away, with miles of densely
wooded hills and valleys between him and his pack.

Do you ask, “How was it possible to know all this?” From the story the
snow told. At daybreak I had found the trail of a hunting pack, and was
following it stealthily, with many a cautious détour and look ahead,
for they are unbelievably shy brutes; and so it happened that I came
upon the carcass of the deer only a few minutes after the wolves had
fed and roamed lazily off toward their day-bed. I followed them too
eagerly, and alarmed them before I could pick the big one I wanted;
whereupon they took to rough country, traveling a pace that left me
hopelessly far behind. When I returned to the deer, to read how the
wolves had surprised and killed their game, I noticed the fresh trail
of a solitary wolf coming in at right angles to the trail of the
hunting pack. It was the limper again, who had just eaten what he
wanted and trailed off by himself. I followed and soon jumped him, and
took after him on the lope, thinking I could run him down or at least
come near enough for a revolver-shot; but that was a foolish notion.
Even on three legs he whisked through the thick timber so much easier
than I could run on snow-shoes that I never got a second glimpse of
him.

By that time I was bound to know, if possible, how the limper happened
to find this second deer for his comfort; so I picked up his incoming
trail and ran it clear back to his den under the windfall, from which
he had come as straight as if he knew exactly where he was heading. His
trail was from eastward; what little air was stirring came from the
south; so that it was impossible for his nose to guide him to the meat
even had he been within smelling distance, as he certainly was not. The
record in the snow was as plain as any other print, and from it one
might reasonably conclude that either the wolves can send forth a
silent food-call, with some added information, or else that a solitary
wolf may be so in touch with his pack-mates that he knows not only
where they are, but also, in a general way, what they are doing.

In comparison with timber-wolves the caribou is rather a witless brute;
but he, too, has his “uncanny” moods, and one who patiently follows
him, with deeper interest in his anima than in his antlered head, finds
him frequently doing some odd or puzzling thing which may indicate a
perception more subtle than that of his dull eyes or keen ears or
almost perfect nose. Here is one example of Megaleep’s peculiar way:

I was trailing a herd of caribou one winter day on the barrens
(treeless plains or bogs) of the Renous River in New Brunswick. For
hours I had followed through alternate thick timber and open bog
without alarming or even seeing my game. The animals were plainly on
the move, perhaps changing their feeding-ground; and when Megaleep
begins to wander no man can say where he will go, or where stop, or
what he is likely to do next. Once, after trailing him eight or ten
miles, twice jumping him, I met him head-on, coming briskly back in his
own tracks, as if to see what was following him. From the trail I read
that there were a dozen animals in the herd, and that one poor wounded
brute lagged continually behind the others. He was going on three legs;
his right forefoot, the bone above it shattered by some blundering
hunter’s bullet, swung helplessly as he hobbled along, leaving its
pathetic record in the snow.

On a wooded slope which fell away to a chain of barrens, halting to
search the trail ahead, my eye caught a motion far across the open, and
through the field-glass I saw my herd for the first time, resting
unsuspiciously on the farther edge of the barren, a full mile or more
away. From my feet the trail led down through a dense fringe of
evergreen, and then straight out across the level plain. A few of the
caribou were lying down; others moved lazily in or out of the forest
that shut in the barren on that side; and as I watched them two
animals, yearlings undoubtedly, put their heads together for a pushing
match, like domestic calves at play.

Hardly had I begun to circle the barren, keeping near the edge of it
but always out of sight in the evergreens, when I ran upon a solitary
caribou trail, the trail of the cripple, who had evidently wearied and
turned aside to rest, perhaps knowing that his herd was near the end of
its journey. A little farther on I jumped him out of a fir thicket, and
watched him a moment as he hobbled deeper into the woods, heading away
to the west. The course surprised me a little, for his mates were
northward; and at the thought I quickly found an opening in the cover
and turned my glass upon the other caribou. Already they were in wild
alarm. For a brief interval they ran about confusedly, or stood tense
as they searched the plain and the surrounding woods for the source of
danger; then they pushed their noses out and racked away at a marvelous
pace, crossing the barren diagonally toward me and smashing into the
woods a short distance ahead, following a course which must soon bring
them and their wounded mate together. If I were dealing with people, I
might say confidently that they were bent on finding out what the alarm
was about; but as I have no means of knowing the caribou motive, I can
only say that the two trails ran straight as a string through the
timber to a meeting-point on the edge of another barren to the
westward.

If you would reasonably explain the matter, remember that these
startled animals were far away from me; that the cripple and myself
were both hidden from their eyes, and that I was moving upwind and
silently. It was impossible that they should hear or see or smell me;
yet they were on their toes a moment after the cripple started up, as
if he had rung a bell for them. It was not the first time I had
witnessed a herd of animals break away when, as I suspected, they had
received some silent, incomprehensible warning, nor was it the last;
but it was the only time when I could trace the whole process without
break or question from beginning to end. And when, to test the matter
to the bottom, I ran the trail of the herd back to where they had been
resting, there was no track of man or beast in the surrounding woods to
account for their flight.

One may explain this as a mere coincidence, which is not an
explanation; or call it another example of the fact that wild animals
are “queer,” which is not a fact; but in my own mind every action of
the caribou and all the circumstances point to a different
conclusion—namely, that the fear or warning or impulse of one animal
was instantly transferred to others at a distance. I think, also, that
the process was not wholly unconscious or subconscious, but that one
animal sent forth his warning and the others acted upon it more or less
intelligently. This last is a mere assumption, however, which cannot be
proved till we learn to live in an animal’s skin.

It is true that the event often befalls otherwise, since you may jump
one animal without alarming others of the same herd; and it is possible
that the degree or quality of the alarm has something to do with its
carrying power, as we feel the intense emotion of a friend more quickly
than his ordinary moods. In this case the solitary caribou was
tremendously startled; for I was very near, and the first intimation he
had of me, or I of him, was when my snow-shoe caught on a snag and I
pitched over a log almost on top of him. Yet the difficulty of drawing
a conclusion from any single instance appears in this: that I have more
than once stalked, killed and dressed an animal without disturbing
others of his kind near at hand (it may be that no alarm was sent out,
for the animal was shot before he knew the danger, and in the deep
woods animals pay little attention to the sound of a rifle); and again,
when I have been trying to approach a herd from leeward, I have seen
them move away hurriedly, silently, suspiciously, in obedience to some
warning which seemed to spread through the woods like a contagion.

The latter experience is common enough among hunters of big game, who
are often at a loss to explain the sudden flight of animals that a
moment ago, under precisely the same outward influences, were feeding
or resting without suspicion. Thus, you may be stalking a big herd of
elk, or wapiti, which are spread out loosely over half a mountainside.
You are keen for the master bull with the noble antlers; nothing else
interests you, more’s the pity; but you soon learn that the cunning old
brute is hidden somewhere in the midst of the herd, depending on the
screen of cow-elk to warn him of danger to his precious skin. Waiting
impatiently till this vanguard has moved aside, you attempt to worm
your way nearer to the hidden bull. You are succeeding beautifully, you
think, when a single cow that you overlooked begins to act uneasily.
She has not seen or heard you, certainly, and the wind is still in your
favor; but there she stands, like an image of suspicion, head up,
looking, listening, testing the air, till she makes up her mind she
would as lief be somewhere else, when without cry or grunt or warning
of any kind that ears can hear she turns and glides rapidly away.

Now if you value animal lore above stuffed skins, or experience above
the babble of hunting naturalists, forget the big bull and his
greed-stirring antlers; scramble quickly to the highest outlook at
hand, and use your eyes. No alarm has been sounded; the vast silence is
unbroken; yet for some mysterious reason the whole herd is suddenly on
the move. To your right, to your left, near at hand or far away, bushes
quiver or jump; alert brown forms appear or vanish like shadows, all
silent and all heading in the direction taken by the first sentinel.
One moment there are scores of elk in sight, feeding or resting
quietly; the next they are gone and the great hillside is lifeless. The
thrill of that silent, moving drama has more wisdom in it, yes, and
more pleasure, than the crash of your barbarous rifle or the convulsive
kicking of a stricken beast that knows not why you should kill him.

Such is the experience, known to almost every elk-hunter who has
learned that life is more interesting than death; and I know nothing of
deer nature to explain it save this—that the whole herd has suddenly
felt and understood the silent impulse to go, and has obeyed it without
a question, as the young wolf or fox cub obeys the silent return call
of his watchful mother.

Such impulses seem to be more common and more dependable among the
whales, which have rudimentary or imperfect sense-organs, but which are
nevertheless delicately sensitive to external impressions, to the
approach of unseen danger, to the movements of the tiny creatures on
which they feed, to changes of wind or tide and to a falling barometer,
as if nature had given them a first-class feeling apparatus of some
kind to make up for their poor eyes and ears. Repeatedly have I been
struck by this extraordinary sensitiveness when watching the monstrous
creatures feeding with the tide in one of the great bays of the
Newfoundland or the Labrador coast. If I lowered a boat to approach one
of them, he would disappear silently before I could ever get near
enough to see clearly what he was doing. That seemed odd to me; but
presently I began to notice a more puzzling thing: at the instant my
whale took alarm every other whale of the same species seemed to be
moved by the same impulse, sounding when the first sounded, or else
turning with him to head for the open sea.

A score of times I tried the experiment, and commonly, but not
invariably, with the same result. I would sight a few leviathans
playing or feeding, shooting up from the deep, breaching half their
length out of water to fall back with a tremendous souse; and through
my glasses I would pick up others here or there in the same bay.
Selecting a certain whale, I would glide rapidly toward him, crouching
low in the dory and sculling silently by means of an oar over the
stern. By some odd channel of perception (not by sight, certainly, for
I kept out of the narrow range of his eye, and a whale is not supposed
to smell or hear) he would invariably get wind of me and go down; and
then, jumping to my feet, I would see other whales in the distance
catch the instant alarm, some upending as they plunged to the deeps,
others whirling seaward and forging full speed ahead.

This observation of mine is not unique, as I supposed, for later I
heard it echoed as a matter of course by the whalemen. Thus, when I
talked with my friend, Captain Rule, about the ways of the great
creatures he had followed in the old whaling-days, he said, “The
queerest habit of a whale, or of any other critter I ever fell foul of,
was this: when I got my boat close enough to a sperm-whale to put an
iron into him, every other sperm-whale within ten miles would turn
flukes, as if he had been harpooned, too.” But he added that he had not
noticed the same contagion of alarm, not in the same striking or
instantaneous way, when hunting the right or Greenland whale—perhaps
because the latter is, as a rule, more solitary in its habits.

Wolves and caribou and whales are far from the observation of most
folk; but the winter birds in your own yard may some time give you a
hint, at least, of the same mysterious transference of an impulse over
wide distances. When you scatter food for them during a cold snap or
after a storm (it is better not to feed them regularly, I think,
especially in mild weather when their proper food is not covered with
snow) your bounty is at first neglected except by the house sparrows
and starlings. Unlike our native birds, these imported foreigners are
easily “pauperized,” seeking no food for themselves so long as you take
care of them. They keep tabs on you, also, waiting patiently about the
house, and soon learn what it means when you emerge from your back door
on a snowy morning with a broom in one hand and a pan in the other.
They are feeding greedily the moment your back is turned, and for a
time they are the only birds at the table. When they have gorged
themselves, for they have no manners, a few tree-sparrows and juncos
flit in to eat daintily. Then suddenly the wilder birds appear—jays,
chickadees, siskins, kinglets and, oh, welcome! a flock of
bob-whites—coming from you know not where, in obedience to a summons
which you have not heard. Some of these may have visited the yard in
time past, and are returning to it now, hunger driven; but others you
have never before met within the city limits, and a few have their
accustomed dwelling in the pine woods, which are miles away. How did
these hungry hermits suddenly learn that food was here?

The answer to that question is simple, and entirely “sensible” if you
think only of birds that live or habitually glean in your neighborhood.
Some of them saw you scatter the food, or else found it by searching,
while others spied these lucky ones feeding and came quickly to join
the feast. For birds that live wider afield there is also an
explanation that your senses can approve, though it is probably wrong
or only half right: from a distance they chanced to see wings speeding
in the direction of your yard, and followed them expectantly because
wings may be as eloquent as voices, the flight of a bird when he is
heading for food being very different from the flight of the same bird
when he is merely looking for food. But these most rare visitors,
kinglets or pine-finches or grosbeaks or bob-whites, that never before
entered your yard, and that would not be here now had you not thought
to scatter food this morning,—at these you shake your head, calling it
chance or Providence or mystery, according to your mood or disposition.
To me, after observing the matter closely many times, the reasonable
explanation of these rare visitors is that either wild birds know how
to send forth a silent food-call or, more likely, that the excitement
of feeding birds spreads powerfully outward, and is felt by other
starving birds, alert and sensitive, at a distance beyond all possible
range of sight or hearing. By no other hypothesis can I account for the
fact that certain wild birds make their appearance in my yard at a
moment when a number of other birds are eagerly feeding, and at no
other time, though I watch for them from one year’s end to another.

Like every other explanation, whether of stars or starlings, this also
leads to a greater mystery. The distance at which such a summoning call
can be felt by others must be straitly limited, else would all the
starving birds of a state be flocking to my yard on certain mornings;
and the force by which the silent call is projected is as unknown as
the rare mental ether which bears its waves or vibrations in all
directions. Yet the problem need not greatly trouble us, since the
answer, when it comes, will be as natural as breathing. If silent or
telepathic communication exists in nature, and I think it surely does,
the mystery before us is no greater than that which daily confronts the
astronomer or the wireless operator. One measures the speed of light
from Orion; the other projects his finger-touch across an ocean; but
neither can tell or even guess the quality of the medium by which the
light or the electric wave is carried to its destination.








V

THE SWARM SPIRIT


This is a chapter on the wing drill of birds, the swarming of bees, the
panics and unseasonal migrations of larger or smaller beasts, and other
curious phenomena in which the wild creatures of a flock or herd all
act in unison, doing the same thing at the same time, as if governed by
a single will rather than by individual motives. If it should turn out
that the single will were expressed in a voice or cry, or even in a
projected impulse, then are we again face to face with our problem of
animal communication.

Of the fact of collective action there is no doubt, many naturalists
having witnessed it; and there is also a strictly orthodox explanation.
Thus, when you see a large flock of crows “drilling” in the spring or
autumn, rising or falling or wheeling all together with marvelous
precision, the ornithologists resolve the matter by saying that the
many crows act as one crow because they follow a “collective impulse”;
that is, because the same impulse to rise or fall or wheel seizes upon
them all at precisely the same moment. And this they tell you quite
simply, as if pointing out an obvious fact of natural history, when in
reality they are showing you the rarest chimera that ever looked out of
a vacuum.

Now the wonderful wing drill of certain birds has something in it which
I cannot quite fathom or understand, not even with a miracle of
collective impulse to help me; yet I have observed two characteristics
of the ordered flight which may help to dispel the fog of assumption
that now envelops it. The first is, that the drill is seen only when an
uncommonly large number of birds of the same kind are gathered
together, on a sunny day of early spring, as a rule, or in the
perfection of autumn weather.

The starlings [3] furnish us an excellent example of this peculiarity.
For months at a stretch you see them about the house, first in pairs,
next in family groups, then in larger companies, made up, I think, of
birds raised in the same neighborhood and probably all more or less
related; but though you watch these companies attentively from dawn to
dusk, you shall never see them going through any unusual wing drill.
Then comes an hour when flocks of starlings appear on all sides,
heading to a common center. They gather in trees here or there about
the edges of a great field or a strip of open beach, all jabbering like
the blackbirds, which they imitate in their cries, flitting about in
ceaseless commotion, but apparently keeping their family or tribal
organization intact. Suddenly, as at a signal, they all launch
themselves toward the center of the field; the hundred companies unite
in one immense flock, and presto! the drill is on. The birds are no
longer individuals, but a single-minded myriad, which wheels or veers
with such precision that the ash of their ten-thousand wings when they
turn is like the flicker of a signal-glass in the sun.

The same characteristic of uncommon numbers holds true of the crows
and, indeed, of all other species of birds, save one, that ever
practise the wing drill. Wild geese when in small companies, each a
family unit, have a regular and beautiful flight in harrow-shaped
formation; but I have never witnessed anything like a wing drill among
them save on one occasion, when a thousand or more of the birds were
gathered together for a few days of frolic before beginning the
southern migration. Nor have I ever seen the drill among thrushes or
warblers or sparrows or terns or seagulls, which sometimes gather in
uncounted numbers, but which do not, apparently, have the same motive
that leads crows or starlings to unite in a kind of rhythmic air-dance
on periodic occasions.

A second marked characteristic of the wing drill is that it is
invariably a manifestation of play or sport, and that the individual
birds, though they keep the order of the play marvelously well, show in
their looks and voices a suppressed emotional excitement. The drill is
never seen when birds are migrating or feeding or fleeing from danger,
though thousands of them may be together at such a time, but only when
they assemble in a spirit of fun or exercise, and their bodily needs
are satisfied, and the weather or the barometer is just right, and no
enemy is near to trouble them. Whatever their motive or impulse,
therefore, it is certainly not universal or even widespread among the
birds, since most of them do not practise the drill; nor is it in the
least like that mysterious impulse which suddenly sets all the
squirrels of a region in migration, or calls the lemmings to hurry over
plain and forest and mountain till they all drown themselves in the
distant sea; for no sooner is the brief drill over than the companies
scatter quietly, each to its own place, and the individual birds are
again alert, inquisitive, well balanced, precisely as they were before.

The drill is seen at its best among the plover, I think; and, curiously
enough, these are the only birds I know that practise it frequently, in
small or large numbers and in all weathers. I have often watched a
flock come sweeping in to my decoys, gurgling like a thousand fifes
with bubbles in them; and never have I met these perfectly drilled
birds, which stay with us but a few hours on their rapid journey from
the far north to the far south, without renewed wonder at their
wildness, their tameness, their incomprehensible ways. That you may
visualize our problem before I venture an explanation, here is what you
may see if you can forget your gun to observe nature with a deeper
interest:

You have risen soon after midnight, called by the storm and the
shrilling of passing plover, and long before daylight you are waiting
for the birds on the burnt-over plain. Your “stand” is a hole in the
earth, hidden by a few berry-bushes; and before you, at right angles
with the course of the storm (for plover always wheel to head into the
wind when they take the ground), are some scores of rudely painted
decoys. As the day breaks you see against the east a motion as of
wings, and your call rings out wild and clear, to be echoed on the
instant. In response to your whistle the distant motion grows wildly
fantastic; it begins to whirl and eddy, as if a wisp of fog were
rolling swiftly down-wind; only in some mysterious fashion the fog
holds together, and in it are curious flickerings. Those are plover,
certainly; no other birds have that perfect unity of movement; and now,
since they are looking for the source of the call they have just heard,
you throw your cap in the air or wave a handkerchief to attract
attention. There is an answering flash of white from the under side of
their wings as the plover catch your signal and turn all at once to
meet it. Here they come, driving in at terrific speed straight at you!

It is better to stop calling now, because the plover will soon see your
decoys; and these birds when on the ground make no sound except a low,
pulsating whistle of welcome or recall. This is uttered but seldom, and
unless you can imitate it, which is not likely, your whistling will do
no good. Besides, it could not possibly be heard. Listen to that
musical babel, and let your nerves dance to it! In all nature there is
nothing to compare for utter wildness with the fluting of incoming
plover.

On they come, hundreds of quivering lines, which are the thin edges of
wings, moving as one to a definite goal. Their keen eyes caught the
first wave of your handkerchief in the distance; and now they see their
own kind on the ground, as they think, and their babel changes as they
begin to talk to them. Suddenly, and so instantaneously that it makes
you blink, there is a change of some kind in every quivering pair of
wings. At first, in the soft light of dawn, you are sure that the
plover are still coming, for you did not see them turn; but the lines
grow smaller, dimmer, and you know that every bird in the flock has
whirled, as if at command, and is now heading straight away. You put
your fingers to your lips and send out the eery plover call again and
again; but it goes unheeded in that tumult of better whistling. The
quivering lines are now all blurred in one; with a final flicker they
disappear below a rise of ground; the birds are gone, and you cease
your vain calling. Then, when you are thinking you will never see that
flock again, a cloud of wings shoot up from the plain against the
horizon; they fall, wheel, rise again in marvelous flight, not as a
thousand individuals but as a unit, and the lines grow larger, clearer,
as the plover come sweeping back to your decoys once more.

Such is the phenomenon as I witnessed it repeatedly on the Nantucket
moors, many years ago. The only way I can explain the instantaneous
change of flight is by the assumption, no longer strange or untested,
that from some alarmed plover on the fringe or at the center of the
flock a warning impulse is sent out, and the birds all feel and obey it
as one bird. That the warning is a silent one I am convinced, for it
seems impossible that any peculiar whistle could be heard or understood
in that wild clamor of whistling. Nor is it a satisfactory hypothesis
that one bird sees the danger or suspects the quality of the decoys,
and all the others copy his swift flight; for in that case there must
be succession or delay or straggling in the turning, and the impression
left on the eye is not of succession, but of almost perfect unity of
movement.

The only other explanation of the plovers’ action is the one commonly
found in the bird-books, to which I have already briefly referred, and
which we must now examine more narrowly. It assumes that all the birds
of a migrating flock are moved not by individual wills, but by a
collective impulse or instinct, which affects them all alike at the
same instant. In support of this favorite theory we are told to
consider the bees, which are said to have no individual motives, and no
need for them, since they blindly follow a swarm or hive instinct that
makes them all precisely alike in their actions. The same swarm
instinct appears often in the birds, but less strongly, because they
are more highly developed creatures, with more need and therefore more
capacity for individual incentive.

This illustration of the hive is offered so confidently and accepted so
readily, as if it were an axiom of natural history, that one hesitates
to disturb the ancient idol in its wonted seat. Yet one might argue
that any living impulse, whether in bees or birds, must proceed from a
living source, and, if that be granted, speculate on the absorbing
business of a nature or a heaven that should be perpetually interfering
in behalf of every earthly flock or swarm or herd by sending the
appropriate impulse at precisely the right moment. And when our
speculation is at an end, I submit the fact that, when I have broken
open a honey-tree in the woods, one bee falls upon the sweets to gorge
himself withal, while another from the same swarm falls angrily upon me
and dies fighting; which seems to upset the collective-impulse idol
completely.

I must confess here that I know very little about bees. They are still
a mystery to me, and I would rather keep silence about them until I
find one bee that I fancy I understand, or one man who offers something
better than a very hazy or mystical explanation of a bee’s
extraordinary action. Yet I have watched long hours at a hive, have
handled a swarm without gloves or mask, and have performed a few
experiments—enough to convince me that the collective-impulse theory
does not always hold true to fact even among our honey-makers. Indeed,
I doubt that it ever holds true, or that there is in nature any such
mysterious thing as a swarm or flock or herd impulse.

In the first place, the bees of the same swarm do not look alike or act
alike except superficially; at least I have not so observed them. Study
the heads or the feet of any two bees under a glass, and you shall find
as much variety as in the heads or feet of any other two creatures of
the same kind, whether brute or human. The lines of difference run
smaller, to be sure; but they are always there. In action also the bees
are variable; they do marvelously wise things at one moment, or
marvelously stupid things at another; but they do not all and always do
the same thing under the same circumstances, for when I have
experimented with selected bees from the same hive I have noticed very
different results; which leads me to suspect that even here I am
dealing with individuals rather than with detached fragments of a
swarm. It is hard, for example, to make a trap so simple that an
imprisoned bee will find his way out of it; but when by great ingenuity
you do at last make a trap so very simple that it seems any creature
with legs must walk out by the open door, perhaps one bee in five will
do the trick; while the other four wait patiently until they die for
more simplicity.

Again, while your eye often sees unity of action among the wild
creatures, neither your reading nor your own reason will ever reveal a
scrap of positive evidence that there is in nature any such convenient
thing (humanly convenient, that is, for explanations) as a swarm or
flock instinct; though, like the mythical struggle for existence, we
are forever hearing about it or building theories upon it. So far as we
know anything about instinct, it is neither collective nor incorporeal.
It is, to use the definition of Mark Hopkins, which is as good as
another and beautifully memorable, “a propensity prior to experience
and independent of instruction.” And the only needful addition to this
high-sounding definition is, that it is a “propensity” lodged in an
individual, every time. It is not and cannot be lodged in a swarm or a
hive; you must either put it into each of two bees or else put it
between them, leaving them both untouched. In other words, the swarm
instinct has logically no abiding-place and no reality; it is a castle
in the air with no solid foundation to rest on.

On its practical or pragmatic side also the theory is a failure, since
the things bees are said to do in obedience to an incorporeal swarm
instinct are more naturally and more reasonably explained by other
causes. Bees swarm, apparently, in the lead or under the influence of
individuals; and it needs only a pair of eyes to discover that there
are plenty of individual laggards and blunderers in the process. They
grow angry not all at once, but successively; not because a swarm
instinct impels them to anger, but because one irritated bee gives off
a pungent odor or raises a militant buzzing, and the others smell the
odor or hear the buzzing and are inflamed by it, each through his own
senses and by the working of his own motives. On a hot day you will see
a few bees fanning air into the hive with their wings, and when these
grow weary others take their places; but if it were a swarm instinct
that impelled them, you would see all the bees fanning or all
sweltering at the same moment. As for the honey-making instinct, on any
early-spring day you will find a few bees working in the nearest
greenhouse, while the others, which are supposed to be governed by the
same collective impulse, are comfortably torpid in the hive or else
eating honey faster than these enterprising ones can make it.

I judge, therefore, that the communistic bees have some individual
notions, and any show of individuality is so at variance with the
common-impulse theory that it seems to illustrate Spencer’s definition
of tragedy, which is, “a theory slain by a fact.” In short, bees have
our common social instinct highly developed, or overdeveloped, and
possibly they have also, like all the higher orders, a stronger or
weaker instinct of imitation; but these are very different matters,
more natural and more consistent with the facts than is the alleged
swarm instinct.

A scientific friend, the most observant ornithologist I have ever met,
has just offered an interesting explanation of the flock or herd
phenomena we are here considering. He finds little evidence of a swarm
instinct, as distinct from our familiar social instinct; but he has
often marveled at the wing drill of birds, and has twice witnessed an
alarm or warning of danger spread silently among a herd of scattered
beasts; and he accounts for the observed facts by the supposition that
the minds (or what corresponds to the minds) of the lower orders are
often moved not from within, but from without—that is, not by instinct
or by sense impressions, not by what they or others of their kind may
see or hear, but by some external and unknown influence. My caribou
rushed away, he thinks, and my incoming plover turned as one bird from
my decoys, because a warning impulse fell upon them at a moment when
they were in danger, but knew it not; and they obeyed it, as they obey
all their impulses, without conscious thought or knowledge of what they
are doing or why they are doing it.

Here is some suggestion of a very modern psychology which is inclined
to regard the mind as a thought-receiving rather than as a
thought-producing instrument, and with that I have some sympathy; but
here is also a rejuvenation of the incorporeal swarm instinct and other
fantastic or romantic notions of animals which preclude observation. If
the anima of a bird or beast is so constituted that it can receive
impulses from a mysterious and unknown source, what is to prevent it
from receiving such silent impulses from another anima like itself? And
why seek an unseen agent for the warning to my caribou or my plover
when one of the creatures saw the danger and was enough moved by it to
sound a mental tocsin?

The trouble with my friend’s explanation, and with all others I have
thus far heard or read, is twofold. First, like the swarm-impulse
theory, it really explains nothing, but avoids one mystery or
difficulty by taking refuge in another. There was a Hindu philosopher
who used to teach, after the manner of his school, that the earth stood
fast because it rested on the back of a great elephant; which was
satisfactory till a thoughtful child asked, “But the elephant, what
does he stand on?” So when I see intelligent caribou or plover fleeing
from an unsensed danger, and am told that they have received an impulse
from without, I am bound to ask, “Where did that impulse come from, and
who sent it?” For emotional impulses do not drop like rain from the
clouds, or fall like apples from unseen trees; they must have their
source in a living, intelligent being of some kind, who must feel the
impulse before sending it to others. No other explanation is humanly
comprehensible.

This leads to the second objection to the theory of external impulse,
and to every other notion of a collective or incorporeal swarm
instinct—namely, that it contradicts all the previous experience of the
wild creature, or at least all educative experience, which lies plain
and clear to our observation. To each bird and animal are given
individual senses, individual wit and a personal anima; and each begins
his mortal experience not in a great flock or herd, but always in
solitary fashion, under the care and guidance of a mother animal that
has a saving knowledge of a world in which the little one is a
stranger. Thus, I watch the innocent fawn when it begins to follow the
wary old doe, or the fledgling snipe as it leaves the nest under expert
guidance, or the wonder-eyed cub coming forth from its den at the call
of the gaunt old she-wolf. In each case I see a mother intelligently
caring for her young, leading them to food, warding them from danger,
calling them now to assemble or now to scatter; and before my eyes
these ignorant youngsters quickly learn to adapt themselves to the
mother’s ways and to obey her every signal. Sometimes I see them
plainly when some manner of silent communication passes among them
(something perhaps akin to that which passes when you catch a friend’s
eye and send your thought or order to him across a crowded room), and
it has even seemed to me, as recorded elsewhere in our observation of
wolf and fox dens, that the young understand this silent communication
more readily than they learn the meaning of audible cries expressive of
food or danger.

Such is the wild creature’s earliest experience, his training to
accommodate himself to the world, and to ways that wiser creatures of
his own kind have found good in the world. When his first winter draws
near he is led by his mother to join the herd or pack or migrating
flock; and he is then ready not for some mysterious new herd or flock
instinct, but for the same old signals that have served well to guide
or warn him ever since he was born. I conclude, therefore, naturally
and reasonably, that my caribou broke away and my incoming plover
changed their flight because one of their number detected danger and
sent forth a warning impulse, which the others obeyed promptly because
they were accustomed to just such communications. There was nothing
unnatural or mysterious or even new in the experience. So far as I can
see or judge, there is no place or need for a collective herd or flock
impulse, and the birds and beasts have no training or experience by
which to interpret such an impulse if it fell upon them out of heaven.

Our human experience, moreover, especially that which befalls on the
borderland of the subconscious world where the wild creatures mostly
live, may give point and meaning to our natural philosophy. There are
emotions, desires, impulses which may be conveyed by shouting; and
there are others which may well be told without shouting, or even
without words. A cheerful man radiates cheerfulness; a strong man,
strength; a brave man, courage (we do not know to what extent or with
what limitations); and a woman may be more irritated by a man who says
nothing than by a man who says too much. These common daily trials may
be as side-lights on the tremendous fact that love, fear, hate,—every
intense emotion is a force in itself, a force to be reckoned with,
apart from the cry or the look by which it is expressed; that all such
emotions project themselves outward; and that possibly, or very
probably, there is some definite medium to convey them, as an unknown
medium which we call “ether” conveys the waves of light.

It is true that we habitually receive such emotional impulses from
others by means of our eyes or ears; but sometimes we apparently imbibe
them through our skin, as Anthony Trollope said he learned Latin, and
once in a way we receive them from another without knowing or thinking
of the process at all. It is noteworthy that the most companionable
people in the world are silent people, especially a silent friend, and
that the silence of any man is invariably more eloquent than his
speech. The silence of one man rests you like a melody; the silence of
another bores you to yawning, perhaps because it is a “dead” silence;
the quietude of a third excites your curiosity to such an extent that,
for once in your life, you behave like a perfectly natural animal; that
is, you go round the silent one, as it were, view him mentally from all
sides, sniff at his opinions from leeward, whir your wings in his face
like a sparrow, or stamp your foot at him like a rabbit—all this to
stir him up and to uncover what interesting thing lies behind his
silence. And why? Simply because every living man is silently,
unconsciously projecting his real thought or feeling, and you are
unconsciously understanding it or else making a vain conscious effort
in that direction.

Such experiences are commonly confined to a room, to the circle of an
open fire; but they are not limited by necessity to any narrow reach,
since there is nothing in a wall to hinder a man’s love or hate from
passing through, or in the air to check its far-going, or in the nature
of another man to prevent its reception. The influence of one person’s
unvoiced will or purpose or warning or summons upon another person at a
distance, should it turn out more common than we now believe possible
because of our habit of speech, would be nothing unnatural or
mysterious, but rather a true working of the subconscious or animal
mind, which had its own way of communication before ever speech was
invented.

Whitman, who sometimes got hold of the tail end of philosophy (and who
was wont to believe he could drag it out, like a trapped woodchuck, and
whirl it around his head with barbaric whoops), was often seen at the
burrow of this thought-transference doctrine:


    These yearnings why are they? these thoughts in the darkness why
        are they?
    Why are there men and women that while they are near me the
        sunlight expands my blood?
    Why when they leave me do my pennants of joy sink flat and lank?
    Why are there trees I never walk under but large and melodious
        thoughts descend upon me?
    What is it that I interchange so suddenly with strangers?
    What with some driver as I ride on the seat by his side?
    What with some fisherman drawing his seine by the shore as I walk
        by and pause?
    What gives me to be free to woman’s and man’s good-will? what gives
        them to be free to mine?


Again, our familiar human experience may throw some clearer light than
ever comes from the laboratory of animal psychologists upon the action
of gregarious brutes in their so-called blind panics, when they are
said to be governed by some extraneous or non-individual herd impulse.
How such a theory originated is a puzzle to one who has closely
observed animals in the open, since their panics are never “blind,” and
their “extraneous” impulse may often be traced to an alarmed animal of
their own kind, or even to an excited human being, whose emotions are
animal-like both in their manifestation and in their irritating effect.
A dog is more easily roused by human than by canine excitement. A
frightened rider sends his fear or irresolution in exaggerated form
into the horse beneath him. The herd of swine that ran down a steep
place into the sea were possessed, I should say, not by exorcised
demons, but by the hysteria received directly from some man or woman of
the excited crowd in the immediate neighborhood. Panic is more
infectious than any fever, and knows no barriers between brute and
human. Indeed, in a frightened crowd in the Subway, in a theater where
smoke appears, or in any other scene of emotional excitement, you may
in a few minutes observe actions more panicky, more suggestive of a
herd impulse (if there be such a fantastic thing in orderly nature),
than can be seen in a whole lifetime of watching wild animals.

In my head at this moment is the vivid impression of a night when I was
caught and carried away by a crowd of Italian socialists, twenty
thousand frenzied men and a few ferocious women, that first eddied like
a storm-tide about the great square under the cathedral at Milan,
howling, shrieking, imprecating, and then poured tumultuously through
choked streets to hurl paving-blocks at the innocent roof of the
railroad station, as at a symbol of government. The roof was of glass,
and the clattering smash of it seemed to get on the nerves of men, like
the cry of sick-em! to an excited dog, rousing them to a senseless fury
of destruction. Clear and thrilling above the tumult a bugle sang, like
a note from heaven, and into the seething mass of humanity charged a
squadron of cavalry, striking left or right with the flats of their
sabers, raising a new hubbub of shrieks and imprecations as the weaker
were trampled down. Fear? That crowd knew no more of fear just then
than an upturned hive of bees. They met the charge with a roar, a
hoarse, solid shout that seemed to sweep the cavalry away like smoke in
the wind. Unarmed men swarmed at the horses like enraged baboons,
hurling stones or curses as they went. The rush ended in a triumphant
yell, and riderless horses, their eyes and nostrils aflame, went
plunging, kicking, squealing through the pandemonium.

There must have been something tremendously animal in the scene, after
all; for when I recall it now I see, as if Memory had carved her statue
of the event, an upreared horse with a crumpled rider toppling from the
saddle; and I hear not the shouts or curses of men, but the horrible
scream of a maddened brute.

It was the night, many years ago, when news of disaster to the Italian
army at Adowa broke loose, after being long suppressed, and I learned
then for the first time what emotional excitement means when the gates
are all down. One had to hold himself against it, as against a flood or
a mighty wind. To yield, to lose self-control even for an instant, was
to find oneself howling, reaching for paving-blocks, seeking an enemy,
lifting a bare fist against charging horse or swinging steel, like the
other lunatics. I caught a man by the shoulders, held him, and bade him
in his own language tell me what the row was about; but he only stared
at me wildly, his mouth open. I caught another, and he struck at my
face; a third, and he shrieked like a trapped beast. Only one gave me a
half-coherent answer, a man whom I dragged from under a saber and
pushed into a side-street. His dear Ambrogio had been conscripted by
the government, he howled (I suppose they had sent his son or brother
with a disaffected Milanese regiment on the African adventure), and
they were all robbers, oppressors, murderers—he finished by jerking
loose from my grasp and hurling himself, yelling, into the mob again.

Had I been a visiting caribou, watching that amazing scene and knowing
nothing of its motive, I might easily have concluded that some
mysterious herd impulse was driving all these creatures to they knew
not what; but, being human, I knew perfectly well that even this
unmanageable crowd had taken its cue from some leader; that the
senseless emotion which inflamed them had originated with individuals,
who had some ground for their passion; and that from the individual the
excitement spread in pestilential fashion until the whole mob caught it
and bent to it, as a field of grass bends to the storm.

Therefore (and I hope you keep the thread of logic through a long
digression), when I go as a man among caribou or wolves or plover or
crows, and see the whole herd or pack or flock acting as one, as if
swayed by a single will, I see no reason why I should evoke an
incorporeal swarm impulse, or “call a spirit from the vasty deep” of
the unknown to explain their similarity of action, since there are
natural causes which may account for the matter perfectly—familiar
causes, too, which still influence men and women as they influence the
remote wood folk.

No, this is not a new animal psychology; it is rather an attempt to
banish the delusion that there is any such thing as a distinct animal
psychology. Science has many forms, and still plenty of delusions, but
there is a basic principle to which she holds steadily—namely, that
Nature is of one piece because her laws are constant. It follows that,
if you know anything of a surety about your own mind, you may
confidently apply the knowledge to any other mind in the universe,
whether in the heavens above or the earth beneath or the waters under
the earth. The only question is, How far may the term “mind” be
properly applied to the brute?

That unanswered question does not immediately concern us, for in
speaking of mind we commonly mean the conscious or reasoning human
article, and we are dealing here with the subconscious mind, which
seems to work after the same fashion whether it appears on two legs or
four. A dog does not know why he becomes excited in a commotion that
does not personally concern him, or why he feels impelled to hasten to
an outcry from an unknown source, or why he looks up, contrary to all
his habits, when everybody else is looking up; and neither does a man
know why he does just such things. Man and brute both act in obedience
to something deeper, more primal and more dependable than reason, and
in this subconscious field they are akin; otherwise it would be
impossible for a man ever to train or to understand a brute, and our
companionable dogs would be as distant as the seraphim.

When, therefore, the same unreasoning actions that are attributed to a
mysterious collective impulse among birds or animals are found among
men to depend on a succession of individual impulses, it is good
psychology as well as good natural history to dismiss the whole herd
instinct as another thoughtless myth. The familiar social and imitative
instincts, the contagion of excitement, the outward projection of
emotional impulses, the sensitive bodily nature of an animal which
enables him to respond to such impulses even when they are
unaccompanied by a voice or cry,—these are comparatively simple and
“sensible” matters which explain all the phenomena of flock or herd
life more naturally and more reasonably.








VI

WHERE SILENCE IS ELOQUENT


Looking back a moment on our trail of animal “talk” before following it
onward, we see, first, that birds and beasts have certain audible cries
which convey a more or less definite meaning of food or danger or
assembly; and second, that they apparently have also some “telepathic”
faculty of sending emotional impulses to others of their kind at a
distance. The last has not been proved, to be sure; we have seen little
more than enough to establish it as a working hypothesis; but whether
we study science or history or an individual bird or beast, it is
better to follow some integrating method or principle than to blunder
around in a chaos of unrelated details. And the hypothesis of silent
communication certainly “works,” since it helps greatly to clarify
certain observed phenomena of animal life that are otherwise darkly
mysterious.

When the same dimly defined telepathic power appears in a man or
woman—so rarely that we are filled with wonder, as in the shadow of a
great mystery or a great discovery—it is not a new but a very old
matter, I think, being merely a survival or reappearance of a faculty
that may have once been in common use among gregarious creatures. All
men seem to have some hint or suggestion of telepathy in them, as shown
by their ability to “speak with their eyes” or to influence their
children by a look; and the few who have enough of it to be conspicuous
receive it, undoubtedly, by some law or freak of heredity, such as
enables one man in a million to wag his ears, or one in a thousand to
follow a subconscious sense of direction so confidently that, after
wandering about the big woods all day, he turns at nightfall and heads
straight for his camp like a homing pigeon. The rest of us, meanwhile,
by employing speech exclusively to express thought or emotion, and by
habitually depending on five senses for all our impressions of the
external world, have not only neglected but even lost all memory of the
gift that once was ours. As an inevitable consequence Nature has taken
her gift away, as she atrophies a muscle that is no longer used, or
devitalizes the nerve of sight in creatures, such as the fishes of
Mammoth Cave, that have lived long time in darkness.

In previous chapters we have noted, as rare examples of telepathy in
human society, that a mother may at times know when an absent son or
husband is in danger, or that an African savage often knows when a
stranger is approaching his village hid in the jungle; but there is
another manifestation of the same faculty which is much more common,
and which we have thus far overlooked, leaving it as an odd and totally
unrelated thing without explanation. I refer to the man, known in
almost every village, who has some special gift for training or
managing animals, who seems to know instinctively what goes on in a
brute’s head, and who can send his own will or impulse into the lower
mind. I would explain that unrelated man, naturally, by the simple fact
or assumption that he has inherited more than usual of the animals’
gift of silent communication.

I knew one such man, a harmless, half-witted creature, who loved to
roam the woods alone by day or night, and whom the wild birds and
beasts met with hardly a trace of the fear or suspicion they manifest
in presence of other human beings. He was always friendly, peaceable,
childlike, and unconsciously or subconsciously, I think, he could tame
or influence these wild spirits by letting them feel his own.

So also could an old negro, an ex-slave, with whom I used to go
fox-hunting in my student days. He could train a dog or a colt in a
tenth part of the time required by ordinary men, and he used no whip or
petting or feeding, or any other device commonly employed by
professional trainers. At times, indeed, his animals acted as if
trained from the moment he touched or spoke to them. He had a mongrel
lot of dogs, cats, chickens, pigs, cows and horses; but they were a
veritable happy family (on a cold night his cats would sleep with a
setting hen, if they could find one, or otherwise with the foxhounds),
and to see them all running to meet “Uncle” when he came home, or
following at his heels or doing what he told them, was to wonder what
strange animal language he was master of.

At daybreak one winter morning I entered the old negro’s kitchen very
quietly, and had a fire going and coffee sending forth its aroma before
I heard him creaking down the stairs. I had traveled “across lots,”
making no sound in the new-fallen snow, and, as I approached the house,
had purposely kept its dark bulk between me and the dogs, which were
asleep in their kennel some distance away. For a time all was quiet as
only a winter dawn can be; but as we sat down to breakfast one of the
hounds with a big bass voice suddenly broke out in an earth-shaking
jubilation. The other hounds quickly caught up the clamor, yelling as
if they had just jumped a fox, while two dogs of another breed were
strangely silent; and then ’Poleon added his bit to the tumult by
stamping, whinnying and finally kicking lustily on the boards of his
stall. ’Poleon, by the way, was an old white horse that Uncle used to
ride (he was “gittin’ too rheumaticky,” he said, to hunt with me
afoot), and this sober beast was fair crazy to join the chase whenever
a fox was afoot.

The negro paid no attention to the noise; but as it went on increasing,
and ’Poleon whinnyed more wildly, and the big-voiced hound kept up a
continuous bellow that might have roused the seven sleepers, the
unseemly racket got on my nerves, so early in the day.

“What the mischief is the matter with Jum this morning?” I demanded.

“Matter? Mischief?” echoed Uncle, as if surprised I did not understand
such plain animal talk. “Why, ol’ Jum’s a-gwine fox-huntin’ dis mawny.
He reckons he knows what we-all’s up to: and now de yother dawgs an’
’Poleon dey reckons dey knows it, too. Jum’s tole um. Dat’s all de
matter an’ de mischief.”

“But how in the world should he know? You never go hunting now unless I
tempt you, and none of the dogs saw or heard me come in,” I objected.

Uncle chuckled at that, chuckled a long time, as if it were a good
joke. “Trust ol’ Jum ter know when we-all’s gwine fox-huntin’,” he
said. “You jes’ trust him. I specks he kinder pick de idee outer de air
soon’s we thunk it, same’s he pick a fox scent. ’Tain’t no use tryin’
ter lie ter Jum, ’cause you can’t fool ’im nohow. No, sir, when dat ol’
dawg’s eroun’, you don’ wanter think erbout nothin’ you don’ want ’im
ter know.”

I had often marveled at Uncle, but now suddenly I thought I understood
him. In his unconscious confession that he thought or felt with his
animals, rather than spoke English to them, was probably the whole
secret of his wonderful gift of training.

The same “secret” is shared by the few men who have the gift of
managing horses, and who can do more by a word or even a look than
another man by bit and harness. I have heard the gift described by a
professional horse-trainer as the “power of the human eye”; but that is
nonsense set to melodrama. An eye is a bit of jelly, and means nothing
without a will or communicating impulse behind it. When the spirit of a
horse is once broken (and most of them are broken rather than trained
by our methods) almost anybody can manage him, the blind as well as the
seeing; but when a horse keeps the spirit of his wild ancestors, their
timidity, their flightiness, their hair-trigger tendency to shy or to
bolt, then I envy the man who can cross the gulf of ages and put
something of his own steadiness into the nervous brute.

This steadying process seems to be wholly a matter of spirit, so far as
I have observed it, and whatever passes from man to brute passes
directly, without need of audible speech. For example, a friend of
mine, a very quiet man of few words, once brought home a magnificent
“blooded” horse which he had bought for a song because “nobody could
handle him.” The horse was not vicious in any way, but seemed to have a
crazy impulse to run himself to death—an impulse so strong that even
now, when he is past twenty years old, he cannot be turned loose for a
moment in a farm pasture. He had never been driven save with a powerful
curb; even so, he would drag the carriage along by the reins, and an
hour of such driving left a strong man’s arms half paralyzed by the
strain. Yet at the first trial his new owner put a soft rubber bit in
his mouth, flipped the lines loosely across his back, and controlled
him by a word.

Some years later I was riding behind that same horse, jogging quietly
along a country road, when my friend, with an odd twinkle in his eye,
said, “Take the reins a moment while I get out this robe.” I took them,
and what followed seemed like magic or bedevilment. I had noticed that
the reins were loose, just “feeling” the horse’s mouth; I shifted them
to my hand very quietly, without stirring a hair, and blinders on the
bridle prevented the horse from seeing the transfer. Yet hardly had I
touched them when something from my hand (or from my soul, for aught I
know) flowed along the leather and filled the brute with fire. He flung
up his head, as if I had driven spurs into him, and was away like a
shot.

Again, I was crossing the public square of Nantucket one morning when I
saw a crowd of excited men and boys eddying at a safe distance around a
horse—an ugly, biting brute that had once almost torn the side of my
face off when I passed too close to him, minding my own affairs. Now he
was having one of his regular tantrums, squealing, kicking, plunging or
backing, while his driver, who had leaped to the ground, alternately
lashed and cursed him. I heard an angry voice near me utter the single
word “Fools!” and saw a stranger brush some men aside and stand
directly in front of the horse, which grew quiet on the instant. The
stranger went nearer, pulled the horse’s head down and laid his face
against it; and there they stood, man and brute, like carved statues.
It was as if one were whispering a secret, and the other listening.
Then the man said, “Come along, boy,” and walked down the square, the
horse following at his heels like a trained dog.

Watching the scene, my first thought was that the horse recognized a
former and kinder master; but the man assured me, when I followed him
up, that he had never spoken to the animal till that moment, and that
he could do the same with any refractory horse he had ever met. “Try it
with that one,” I said promptly, pointing to a nervous horse that,
feeling the excitement of the recent affair, was jerking and frothing
at his hitch-rope. The man smiled his acceptance of the challenge,
stepped in front of the horse, and looked at him steadily. What he
thought or willed, what feeling or impulse he sent out, I know not; but
certainly some silent communication passed, which the horse recognized
by forward-pointing ears and a low whinny of pleasure. Then the man
unsnapped the rope from the bridle ring, turned away without a word,
and the horse followed him across the square and back again to the
hitching-post.

When I asked how the thing was done, the man answered with entire
frankness that he did not know. It “just came natural” to him, he said,
to understand horses, and he had always been able to make them do
almost anything he wanted. But he had no remarkable power over other
animals, so far as I could learn, and was uncommonly shy of dogs, even
of little dogs, regarding them all alike as worthless or dangerous
brutes.

Some of my readers may recall, in this connection, the shabby-genteel
old man who used to amuse visitors in the public gardens of Paris by
playing with the sparrows, some twenty-odd years ago. So long as he
went his way quietly the birds paid no more attention to him than to
any other stroller; but the moment he began to chirp some wild and
joyous excitement spread through the trees. From all sides the sparrows
rushed to him, alighting on his hat or shoulders, clamoring loudly for
the food which they seemed to know was in his pockets, but which he
would not at first give them. When he had a crowd of men and women
watching him (for he was vain of his gift, and made a small living by
passing his hat after an entertainment) he would single out a
cock-sparrow from the flock and cry, “What! you here again, Bismarck,
you scoundrel?” Then he would abuse the cock-sparrow, calling him a
barbarian, a Prussian, a mannerless intruder who had no business among
honorable French sparrows; and finally, pretending to grow violently
angry, he would chase Bismarck from bench to bench and throw his hat at
him. And Bismarck would respond by dodging the hat, chirping blithely
the while, as if it were a good joke, and would fly back to peck at the
crust of bread which the old man held between his lips or left sticking
out of his pocket.

One might have understood this as a mere training trick if Bismarck
were always the same; but he was any cock-sparrow that the man chanced
to pick out of a flock. After playing with the birds till they wearied
of it, he would feed them, pass the hat, and stroll away to repeat his
performance with another flock in another part of the gardens. That
these wary and suspicious birds, far more distrustful of man than the
sparrows of the wilderness, understood his mental attitude rather than
his word or action; that he could make them feel his kindliness, his
camaraderie, his call to come and play, even while he pretended to
chase them,—this was the impression of at least one visitor who watched
him again and again at his original entertainment.

Some kind of communication must have passed silently between the actor
and one of his audience; for presently, though I never spoke to the old
man, but only watched him keenly, he picked me out for personal
attention. Whereupon I cultivated his acquaintance, invited him to dine
and fed him like a duke, and thought I had gained his confidence by
taking him to see a big wolf of mine that might well have puzzled any
student of birds or beasts. The wolf was one of a wild pack that had
recently arrived at the zoo from Siberia, where they had been caught in
a pit and shipped away with all their savagery in them. Through some
freak of nature this one wolf had attached himself to me, like a lost
dog; by some marvelously subtle perception he would recognize my coming
at a distance, even in a holiday crowd, and would thrust his grim
muzzle against the bars of his cage to howl or roar till I came and
stretched out a hand to him, though he was as wild and “slinky” as the
rest of the pack to everybody else, even to the keeper who fed him.
That interested the sparrow-tamer, of course; but he was silent or too
garrulous whenever I approached the thing I wanted to know. He would
not tell me how he won the birds, but made a mystery and hocus-pocus of
the natural gift by which he earned a precarious living.

The same “mystery” cropped out later, amid very different scenes, in
the interior of Newfoundland. Coming down beside a salmon river one
day, my Indian, a remarkable man with an almost uncanny power of
calling wild creatures of every kind, pointed to a hole high on the
side of a stub, and said, “Go, knock-um dere; see if woodpecker at
home.” I went and knocked softly, but nothing happened. “Knock-um
again, knock-um little louder,” said Matty. I knocked again, more
lustily, and again nothing happened. Then the Indian came and rapped
the tree with his knuckles, while I stood aside; and instantly a
woodpecker that was brooding her eggs stuck her head out of the hole
and looked down at her visitor inquisitively.

The next day at the same place we repeated the same performance
precisely, after our morning fishing; and again the interesting thing
to me was, not the bird’s instant appearance at the Indian’s summons,
but the curiously intent way in which she turned her head to look down
at him. When he showed his craft again and again, at the doors of other
woodpeckers that were not interested in my knocking, I demanded, “Now,
Matty, tell me how you do it.”

But Matty only laughed. When we are alone in the woods he has a fine
sense of humor, though grim enough at other times. “Oh, woodpecker know
me; he look down at me,” he said; and that was all I could ever get out
of him.

So, though I have seen the gift in operation several times, I have not
yet found the man who had it and who could or would give me any
explanation. There is no doubt in my own mind, however, that the negro,
the Frenchman, and the Indian, and all others who exercise any unusual
influence over animals, do so by reason of their subconscious power to
“talk” or to convey impulses without words, as gregarious wild
creatures commonly talk among themselves. At least, I can understand
much of what I see among birds and brutes by assuming that they talk in
this fashion.



Such a power seems mysterious, incredible, in a civilized world of
sense and noise; but I fancy that every man and woman speaks silently
to the brute without being conscious of the fact. “If you want to see
game, leave your gun at home,” is an accepted saying among hunters; but
the reason for the excellent admonition has not been forthcoming. When
you have hunted six days in vain, and then on a quiet Sunday stroll
come plump upon noble game that seems to have no fear, you are apt to
think of the curiosities of luck, but even here also are you under the
sway of psychological law and order. As you go quietly through the
woods, projecting your own “aura” of peace or sympathy, it may be, in
an invisible wave ahead of you, there is nothing disturbing or
inharmonious in either your thoughts or your actions; and at times the
wild animal seems curiously able to understand the one as well as the
other, just as certain dogs know at first glance whether a stranger is
friendly or hostile or afraid of them. When you are excited or lustful
to kill, something of your emotional excitement seems to precede you;
it passes over many wild birds and beasts, all delicately sensitive,
before you come within their sense range; and when you draw near enough
to see them you often find them restless, suspicious, though as yet no
tangible warning of danger has floated through the still woods. At the
first glimpse or smell of you they bound away, your action in hiding or
creeping making evident the danger which thus far was only vaguely
felt. But if you approach the same animals gently, without mental
excitement of any kind, sometimes, indeed, they promptly run away,
especially in a much-hunted region; but more frequently they meet you
with a look of surprise; they move alertly here or there to get a
better view of you, and show many fascinating signs of curiosity before
they glide away, looking back as they go.

Such has been the illuminating experience of one man, at least,
repeated a hundred times in the wilderness. I have been deep in the
woods when my food-supply ran low, or was lost in the rapids, or went
to feed an uninvited bear, and it was then a question of shoot game or
go hungry; but the shooting was limited by the principle that a wild
animal has certain rights which a man is bound to respect. I have
always held, for example, that a hunter has no excuse for trying long
shots that are beyond his ordinary skill; that it is unpardonable of
him to “take a chance” with noble game or to “pump lead” after it,
knowing as he does that the chances are fifty to one that, if he hits
at all, he will merely wound the animal and deprive it of that gladness
of freedom which is more to it than life. So when I have occasionally
gone out to kill a buck (a proceeding which I heartily dislike) I have
sometimes hunted for days before getting within close range of the
animal I wanted. But when, in the same region and following the same
trails, I have entered the big woods with no other object than to enjoy
their stillness, their fragrance, their benediction, it is seldom that
I do not find plenty of deer, or that I cannot get as near as I please
to any one of them. More than once in the woods I have touched a wild
deer with my hand (as recorded in another chapter) and many times I
have had them within reach of my fishing-rod.

It is even so with bear, moose, caribou and other creatures—your best
“shots” come when you are not expecting them, and it is not chance, but
psychological law, which determines that you shall see most game when
you leave your gun at home. A hunter must be dull indeed not to have
discovered that the animal he approaches peaceably, trying to make his
eyes or his heart say friendly things, is a very different animal from
the one he stalks with muscles tense and eyes hard and death in the
curl of his trigger finger.

I once met an English hunter, a forest officer in India, who told me
that for the first year of his stay in the jungle he was “crazy” to
kill a tiger. He dreamed of the creatures by night; he hunted them at
every opportunity and in every known fashion by day; he never went
abroad on forest business without a ready rifle; and in all that time
he had just one glimpse of a running tiger. One day he was led far from
his camp by a new bird, and as he watched it in a little opening,
unarmed and happy in his discovery, a tiger lifted its huge head from
the grass, not twenty steps away. The brute looked at him steadily for
a few moments, then moved quietly aside, stopped for another look, and
leaped for cover.

That put a new idea into the man’s head, and the idea was emphasized by
the fact that the unarmed natives, who had no desire to meet a tiger,
were frequently seeing the brutes in regions where he hunted for them
in vain. As an experiment he left his rifle at home for a few months;
he practised slipping quietly through the jungle without physical or
mental excitement, as the natives go, and presently he, too, began to
meet tigers. In one district he came close to four in as many months,
and every one acted in the same half-astonished, half-inquisitive way.
Then, thinking he understood his game, he began to carry his rifle
again, and had what he called excellent luck. The beautiful tiger skins
he showed me were a proof of it.

To me this man was a rare curiosity, being the only Indian or African
hunter I ever met who went into the jungle alone, man fashion, and who
did not depend on unarmed natives or beaters or trackers for finding
his game. His excellent “luck” was, as I judge, simply a realization of
the fact that human excitement may carry far in the still woods, and be
quite as disturbing, as the man-scent or the report of a rifle.

Does all this sound strange or incredible to you, like a chapter from a
dream-book? However it may sound, it is the crystallized conviction
resulting from years of intimate observation of wild beasts in their
native woods; and if you consider it a moment without prejudice, it may
appear more natural or familiar, like a chapter from life. If the man
who sits opposite you can send his good or evil will across a room, so
that you feel his quality without words, or if he can so express
himself silently when he enters your gate that certain dogs instantly
take his measure and welcome or bite him, it is not at all improbable
that the same man can project the same feelings when he goes afield, or
that sensitive wild creatures can understand or “feel him out” at a
considerable distance.

To weigh that probability fairly you must first get rid of your ancient
hunting lore. Hunters are like the Medes and Persians in that they have
laws which alter not; and I suppose if you met ancient Nimrod in the
flesh, his admonition would be, “Keep to leeward and stalk carefully,
breaking no twig, for your game will run away if it winds or hears
you.” That is the first rule I learned for big-game hunting, and it is
founded on fact. But there are two other facts I have observed these
many years, which Nimrod will never mention: the first, that when you
are keenly hunting, it often happens that game breaks away in alarm
before it winds or sees or hears you; and the second, that when you are
not hunting, but peaceably roving the woods, going carelessly and
paying no attention to the wind, you often come very close to wild
game, which stops to watch you curiously after it has seen you and
heard your step or voice and sampled your quality in the air. These two
facts, implying some kind of mental or emotional contact between the
natural man and the natural brute, are probably not accidental or
unrelated, and we are here trying to find the natural law or principle
of which they are the occasional and imperfect expression.

This whole matter of silent communication may appear less strange if we
remember that most wild creatures are all their lives accustomed to
matters which sense-blinded mortals find mysterious or incredible. Why
a caterpillar, which lives but a few hours when all the leaves are
green, should make a cocoon of a single leaf and with a thread of silk
bind that leaf to its stem before he wraps himself up in it, as if he
knew that every leaf must fall; or why a spider, adrift for the first
time on a chip, should immediately send out filaments on the air
currents and, when one of his filaments cleaves to something solid
across the water, pull himself and his raft ashore by it; or why a
young bear, which has never seen a winter, should at the proper time
prepare a den for his long winter sleep,—a thousand such matters, which
are as A B C to natural creatures, are to us as incomprehensible as
hieroglyphics to an Eskimo. That a sensitive animal should know by
feeling (that is, by the reception of a silent message) whether an
approaching animal is in a dangerous or a harmless mood is really no
more remarkable than that he should know, as he surely does, when it is
time for him to migrate or to make ready his winter quarters.

This amazing sensitiveness, resulting, I think, from the reception of a
wordless message, was brought strongly home to me one day as I watched
a flock of black mallards, forty or fifty of them, resting in the
water-grass within a few yards of my hiding-place. A large hawk had
appeared at intervals, circling over the marshes and occasionally over
the pond; but, beyond turning an eye upward when he came too near, the
ducks apparently paid no attention to him. He was their natural enemy;
they had paid toll of their number to satisfy his hunger; but now,
though plainly seen, he was no more regarded or feared than a
dragon-fly buzzing among the reeds. Presently another hawk appeared in
the distance, circling above the meadows. As a wider swing brought him
over the pond a watchful duck uttered a single low quock! On the
instant heads came from under wings; a few ducks shot into the open
water for a look; others sprang aloft without looking, and the whole
flock was away in a twinkling. I think the hawk did not see or suspect
them till they rose in the air, for at the sudden commotion he swooped,
checked himself when he saw that he was too late, and climbed upward
where he could view the whole marsh again.

Now these two hawks were of the same species, and to my eyes they were
acting very much alike. One was hungry, on the lookout for food; the
other was circling for his own amusement after having fed; and though
the eyes of birds are untrustworthy in matters of such fine
distinction, in some way these ducks instantly knew or felt the
difference between the mood of one enemy and that of another. Likewise,
when I have been watching deer in winter, I have seen a doe throw up
her head, cry an alarm and bound away; and her action became
comprehensible a few moments later when a pack of hunting wolves broke
out of the cover. But I have watched deer when a pack of wolves that
were not hunting passed by in plain sight, and beyond an occasional
lift of the head for an alert glance the timid creatures showed no sign
of alarm, or even of uneasiness, in presence of their terrible enemies.

I say confidently that one wolf pack was hunting and the other not
hunting because the northern timber-wolf naturally (that is, in a wild
state and dealing with wild animals) hunts or kills only when he is
hungry. I ran the trails of both packs, and one showed plainly that the
wolves were in search of food; while the other said that the brutes
were roaming the country idly, lazily; and when I ran the back trail of
this second pack I found where they had just killed and eaten. The deer
were not afraid of them because they were for the time quite harmless.

At first I thought that these ducks and deer perceived the mood of
their enemies in a simple way through the senses; that they could infer
from the flight of a hawk or the trot of a wolf whether he were
peaceable or dangerous; and at times this is probably the true
explanation of the matter. The eyes of most birds and beasts, strangely
dull to objects at rest, are instantly attracted to any unusual motion.
If the motion be quiet, steady, rhythmical, they soon lose interest in
it, unless it be accompanied by a display of bright color; but if the
motion be erratic, or if it appear and disappear, as when an
approaching animal hides or creeps, they keep sharp watch until they
know what the motion means or until timidity prompts them to run away.
Thus, chickens or ducks show alarm when a kite slants up into the air;
they lose interest when the kite sits in the wind, and become alert
again when it begins to dive or swoop. It is noticeable, also, that on
a windy day all game-birds and animals are uncommonly wild and
difficult of approach, partly because the constant motion of leaves or
grass upsets them, and partly (in the case of animals) because their
noses are at fault, the air messages being constantly broken up and
confused. But such a “sensible” explanation, suitable as it may be for
times or places, no longer satisfies me, and simply because it does not
explain why on a quiet day game should be unconcerned in presence of
one hawk or wolf, and take to instant flight on the appearance of
another enemy of the same species.

It should be noted here that these “fierce” birds and beasts are no
more savage in killing grouse or deer than the grouse is savage in
eating bugs, or the deer in seeking mushrooms at the proper season;
that they simply seek their natural meat when they are hungry, and that
they are not bloodthirsty or ferocious or wanton killers. Only men, and
dogs trained or spoiled by men, are open to that charge. The birds and
beasts of prey when not hungry (which is a large part of the time,
since they feed but once a day or sometimes at longer intervals) live
as peaceably as one could wish. After feeding they instinctively seek
to be with their own kind and very rarely attempt to molest other
creatures. At such times, when they are resting or playing or roving
the woods, the smaller wood folk pay no more attention to them than to
harmless fish-hawks or porcupines. [4] Repeatedly I have watched
game-birds or animals when their enemies were in sight, and have
wondered at their fearlessness. The interesting question is, how do
they know, as they seemingly do, when the full-fed satisfaction of
their enemy changes to a dangerous mood? Why, for example, are deer
alarmed at the yelp of a she-wolf calling her cubs to the trail, and
why do they feed confidently in the dusk-filled woods, as I have seen
them do, when the air shivers and creeps to the clamor of a wolf pack
baying like unleashed hounds in wild jubilation?

I have no answer to the question, and no explanation except the one
suggested by human experience: that the hunting animal, like the
hunting man, probably sends something of his emotional excitement in a
wave ahead of him, and that some animals are finely sensitive enough to
receive this message and to be vaguely alarmed by it.



The mating of animals, especially the calling of an unseen mate from a
great distance, brings us face to face with the same problem, and
perhaps also the same answer. Sometimes the mating call is addressed to
the outer ear, as in the drumming of a cock-grouse or the whine of a
cow-moose; but frequently a mate appears when, so far as we can hear,
there is no audible cry to call him. How do the butterflies, for
example, know when or where to seek their other halves? That their
meeting is by chance or blunder or accident is a theory which hardly
endures an hour’s observation. In the early spring I take a cocoon from
a certain corner of shrubbery and carry it to my house, and there keep
it till the end softens, when I put it into a box with a screened top
and hang it out under the trees. Presently a gorgeous moth crawls out
of the cocoon; and hardly has she begun to wave her wings to dry them
when the air over the screen is brilliant with dancing wings, the wings
of her would-be mates. And the thing is more puzzling to me because I
have never found a cocoon of that kind in my immediate neighborhood;
nor have I seen a single cecropia this season until the captive called
them.

How they find her so promptly is a problem that I cannot solve. It may
be that the call is wholly physical or sensible, that some fine dust or
aroma is sent forth on the air currents, and the sensitive nerves of
other moths receive and respond to it; but it is still amazing that
wind-blown creatures can follow an invisible air-trail through what
must be to them a constant tempest and whirlwind of air currents, until
they come unerringly to the one desired spot in a limitless universe. I
have shown that pretty sight of dancing wings to many audiences, after
predicting what would happen; and always they saw it with wonder, as if
there were magic in it.

The moth mystery may be dissolved by some such purely physical formula;
but what physical sense will explain the fact that when I turned a
modest hen-pheasant loose in the spring, in a region where my
wide-ranging setter and I never discovered a pheasant, she was
immediately joined by a gloriously colored mate, and soon there was a
hidden nest and then young pheasants to watch? Most birds and beasts
are questing widely in the mating season, and their senses seem to be
more keen at this time, or more concentrated on a single object. On
grounds of what we thoughtlessly call chance, therefore, they would be
more apt to find mates when they are keenly looking for them; but
giving them every possible chance in a wide region where the species is
almost extinct, and then multiplying that chance a hundred times, I
still find it hard to believe that the meeting of two rare animals is
either accidental or the result of ordinary sense-perception. Out of
several examples that occur to me, here are two which especially
challenge the attention:

One early spring a she-fox was caught in her den, some five miles from
the village where I then harbored. She was carefully bagged, carried a
few rods to an old wood road, placed in a wagon and driven over country
highways to the village, where she was confined in a roomy pen in a
man’s dooryard. A few nights later came a snowfall, and in the morning
there were the tracks of a male fox heading straight to the vixen and
making a path round about her pen. She was his mate, presumably, and
when we found his tracks our first feeling of admiration at his
boldness was soon replaced by the puzzling question of how he had found
her so quickly and so surely. To answer that question, if possible, I
followed his back trail.

Now the trail of a fox in the wilderness, where he is sometimes hunted
by wolves or other hungry prowlers, is a bewildering succession of
twistings and crisscrosses; in a settled region, where his natural
enemies are extinct, his trail is bolder, more straightforward, easier
to read; and in either case you can quickly tell by the “signs” whether
your fox is male or female, whether hunting or roaming, or hungry or
satisfied. Also you can tell whether he is just “projeckin’ around,” as
Uncle Remus says, or whether his mind is set on going somewhere. In the
latter event he almost invariably follows runways, or fox roads, which
are as well known to him as are footpaths and stream-crossings to a
country lad. But the trail of this particular fox was different from
any other that I ever followed. That he was a male and was “going
somewhere” was evident enough; but he was not following runways or
paying any attention to them. He left no signs at places where any
ordinary dog-fox would surely have left them, and he was stopping to
listen or to ward himself at uncommonly frequent intervals. So, running
it backward, I read the story of his journey mile after mile, till the
oncoming trail changed to the devious, rambling trot of a questing fox;
and beyond that I had no interest in it.

The place where the fox seemed to have found his bearings, or where he
stopped his rambling to head straight for his mate, was some four miles
distant from the captive in a bee-line. The course he took was entirely
different from that taken by the man who brought the vixen home, thus
excluding the theory that he followed the trail by scent; and the
latter part of his way led through the outskirts of a village, where
the track of a fox had not been seen for many years. From the distant
hills he had come down through sheltering woods at a stealthy trot;
across open pastures on the jump; over a bridge and along a highway,
where he traveled behind a friendly stone wall; then very cautiously
through lanes and garden fringes, where the scent of men and dogs met
him at every turn; turning aside here for a difficulty or there for a
danger, but holding his direction as true as if he followed a compass,
till he came at last with delicate steps to where his mate was silently
calling him. For except on the assumption that she called him, and with
a cry that was soundless, I know not how to explain the fact that he
found her in a place where neither he nor she had ever been before.

It is possible, you may reason, that this was not his first visit; that
unknown to us, venturing among his human and canine enemies, he had by
a lucky chance stumbled upon his mate on an evening when the bare
ground did not betray his secret to our eyes; and that for his next
visit he had cunningly laid out a different trail through manifold
dangers. It was the latter trail, made without doubt or question of
what lay at the end of it, which I had followed in the telltale snow.

That is a good armchair argument, but a very doubtful explanation of
the fox action, since it calls for more reasoning power than we
commonly find in a brute. Remember that this male fox was far away on
his own affairs when his mate was captured, and he had no means of
knowing where she had gone: he simply missed her from her accustomed
haunts, and sought till he found her. Remember also that a male fox is
never allowed to come near his mate’s den; that when she is heavy with
young, as this vixen was when caught, he may join her of an evening and
hunt for her, or bring her food that he has killed, but always, I
think, at a distance from the place where she intends to bring forth
her cubs. That the male, after missing his mate and yapping for her in
vain through the woods, should at last seek her at the forbidden den,
and there find the scent of men and conclude that they had taken her
away; that he should follow the scent of a wagon-wheel over five miles
of country roads, or else explore all the neighboring villages till he
found what he sought; that he should then lay out a different trail,
more secret and more reasonably safe, for his second visit,—all this is
to me more ingenious, more unnatural and more incredible than that his
mate should silently call him, as his mother had probably many times
called him from the den when he was a cub, and that he should feel and
answer her summons.

Such reasoning is purely speculative, but there are certain facts which
we must keep in mind if we are to explain the matter. The first, a
general fact which is open to observation, is that it is fox nature at
certain seasons to come to a captive; for what reason or with what
self-forgetful motive it would be hard to say. I have known mother
foxes and mother wolves to come where their cubs were imprisoned by
men. I have heard a straight record of one male wolf that appeared at a
ranch the second night after his wounded mate was captured by the
ranchman. And I have seen a male fox come to the rescue of a female
when she was driven by dogs and too heavy with young to make a long
run, and wait beside her trail till the dogs appeared, and then lead
them off after him while she made her escape. The second fact, which
may imply some power of silent communication, is that when snow fell
about the pen of this captive fox, a few nights after she was taken,
there were the tracks to show that her mate already knew where she was.
That he found and came to her in the midst of his enemies may be quite
as significant as how he found her, by way of giving a new direction or
interest to our skin-and-bones study of natural history.

In this first example the fox was perhaps moved by the mating impulse,
which sharpened his wit and encouraged his will; but at times a wild
creature may seek and find his mate when no such “call of the blood”
urges him, when he comes with his life in his hand in response to some
finer or nobler motive, something perhaps akin to loyalty, which is the
sum of all virtues. Witness the following:

A friend of mine was hunting one October day when his shot wing-tipped
a quail, apparently the old female of the flock, which his dog caught
and brought to him almost uninjured. What to do was the next question.
It is easy, because thoughtless, to cut down a bird in swift flight;
but when the little thing nestles down in your hand or tries to hide
under your fingers; when you can feel its rapid heart-beat, and its
eyes are big and wondrous bright,—well, then some hunters bite the
head, and some wring the neck, and some would for the moment as lief be
shot as to do either. So to avoid the difficulty my friend put the
quail carefully away in a pocket of his hunting-coat, and brought her
home with some vague idea of taming her, and some dream of trapping a
mate in the spring and perhaps raising some little bob-whites of his
own. At night he put the captive into a coop just inside the barn
window, which was open wide enough to admit air but not a prowling cat;
for he was already beginning to learn that a quail is a most lovable
little pet, and he was bound that she should not again be hurt or
frightened.

Before sunrise next morning he heard low, eager whistlings in the yard;
and there was another quail, a male bob-white, where never one was seen
before nor since. He was perched warily on the window-sill of the barn,
looking in at his captive mate, telling her in the softest of quail
tones (for there were enemies all about) that he had found her and was
glad; while from within the barn came a soft piping and gurgling which
seemed to speak welcome and reassurance. The opening of a door
frightened him; he buzzed away to the orchard, and presently from an
apple-tree came the exquisite quoi-lee! quoi-lee! the assembly-call of
the bob-white family.

The first quail had been caught miles away from the man’s house; there
were no other birds of her own kind within hearing distance, so far as
my friend and his dog ever discovered, and it was not the mating-time,
when quail are questing widely. By a process of elimination, therefore,
one reaches the conclusion that the male bird was, in all probability,
the father of the flock over which the captive presided; that he had
helped to raise the young birds, as quail commonly do; that he had
stood by his family all summer in loyal bob-white fashion, and that he
went out to seek his vanished mate when she failed to answer his
calling.

All that seems clear enough, and in perfect accord with quail habits;
but when you ask, “How did he find her?” there is no answer except the
meaningless word “chance” or a frank admission of our original premise:
that wild birds and beasts all exercise a measure of that mysterious
telepathic power which reappears now and then in some sensitive man or
woman. It may be for the wood folk that “There is no speech nor
language: their voice is not heard,” as the Psalmist wrote of the
communing day and night; but they certainly communicate in some way,
and the longer one studies them the more does it appear that part of
their “talk” is of a finer character than that which our ears can hear.








HOW TO KNOW THE WOOD FOLK


VII

ON GETTING ACQUAINTED


To know birds and beasts may be a greater or a lesser triumph than to
know ornithology or zoölogy. That is a question of taste or
temperament, the only certainty being that the two classes of subjects
are altogether different. The latter deals with external matters, with
form, classification, generalities. Its materials are books, specimens,
museums, one as dead or desiccate as another; and because it is limited
and exact, you can memorize its outlines in a few days, or become in a
few years an authority in the science.

The former subject, of birds and beasts, deals with an endless and
fascinating mystery. Its materials are living and joyous individuals,
among whom are no classes or species, concerning whom there can be no
“authority”; and when, after a lifetime of study, you have made a small
beginning of knowledge, you find that, like All Gaul of misty memory,
it may be divided into three parts.

One part is observation, which is a simple matter of the eye. Another
is sympathy, which belongs to the mind or heart. In dealing with wild
creatures, as with civilized folk, one learns to appreciate De
Quincey’s rule of criticism, “Not to sympathize is not to understand.”
A third part, more rare and variable, may come from that penetrating
but indescribable quality which we call a gift. A few men have it; the
animals instinctively trust them, and they understand animals without
knowing how they understand. The rest, lacking it, must struggle
against a handicap to learn, substituting the slow wisdom of experience
for the quick insight of the gift. It is for the latter chiefly that I
write these wood notes.

One word more by way of preface, to express the conviction that you can
learn nothing worth knowing about birds or beasts so long as you seek
them with a gun in your hand. On that road you shall find only common
dust, and at the end of it a valley of dry bones. Whether you carry the
gun frankly for sport, or delude yourself with the notion that you can
add to natural history by collecting more skins or skulls, you have
unconsciously placed destruction above fulfilment, stark death above
the beautiful mystery of life. So must you estrange both the animal and
yourself, making it impossible for you to meet on any common ground of
understanding. And now for our lessons:



If I were to formulate rules for a subject which can never be learned
by the book, I might say that there are three things you should know,
and another you must do, if you expect to gain any intimate knowledge
of the wood folk, or even to approach them near enough for fair and
leisurely observation.

The first thing to know is that natural creatures, though instinctively
shy or timid, are not wildly governed by fears and terrors, as we have
been misinformed from our youth up. The “reign of terror” is another of
those pet scientific delusions, like the “struggle for existence,” for
which there is no basis in nature. Fear in any true sense of the word
is an exclusively human possession, or affliction; it is a physical and
moral poison, as artificial as sin, which the animal escapes by virtue
of being natural. It is doubtful, indeed, whether anything remotely
resembling our fear, a state of mind arising from a highly developed
imagination which enables us to picture events before they happen, is
ever born into a hairy skin or hatched out of an egg. The natural
timidity of all wild creatures is a protective and wholesome instinct,
radically different from the fear which makes cowards of men who have
learned to trace causes and to anticipate consequences.

So much for the mental analysis; and your eyes emphasize the same
conclusion when you look frankly upon the natural world. The very
attitude or visible expression of birds or beasts when you meet them in
their native woods, feeding, playing, resting, seeking their mates, or
roving freely with their little ones (all pleasurable matters,
constituting nine-tenths or more of animal existence), is enough in
itself to refute the absurd notion of a general reign of terror in
nature. If you are wise, therefore, you will get rid of that prejudice,
or at least hold it in abeyance till the animals themselves teach you
how senseless it is. To go out obsessed with the notion of fear is to
blind your eyes to the great comedy of the woods.

The second thing to know, and to remember when you go forth to see, is
that sensitive creatures dislike to be watched, and become uneasy when
they find a pair of eyes intently fixed upon them. You yourself retain
something of this ancient animal inheritance, it seems, since there is
nothing which more surely excites alarm if you are timid, or challenge
if you are well balanced, or anger if you have a fighting spirit, than
to have a stranger watching your every move while you go about your
lawful affairs. The fact that you cannot word a reason for your alarm
or challenge or anger makes you all the more certain that you have an
unanswerable reason; which is your inborn right to be let alone.

This natural and inalienable right (which society curbs for its own
protection, and reform societies trample on for their peculiar
pleasure) may help you to understand why the animal becomes alarmed
when he finds you watching him closely. He desires above all things
else, above his dinner even, to be let alone; and your eye may as
surely disturb his peace, his self-possession, his sense of security,
as any gun you may shoot at him or any fire you may kindle in his
fragrant domain.

You have but to think a moment in order to understand why even your
look may be too disturbing. When a beast of prey sees a buck that he
wants to catch, what is his invariable mode of procedure? First he
hides, then he creeps or skulks or waits, all the while keeping his
eyes fastened upon his victim, watching every move with fierce
intensity till the moment comes to spring. It follows, naturally
enough, that when the same beast of prey finds other eyes fixed upon
himself, he knows well what the look means, that a rush will swiftly
follow; and he anticipates the rush by taking to his heels. Or the
buck, having once escaped the charge of a hunting beast, will remember
his experience the next time he finds himself an object of scrutiny,
and will flee from it as from any other discomfort.

Whether this action is the result of instinctive or deductive knowledge
is here of no consequence: let the psychologists pick a bone over it.
Since we have in our heads a strong aversion to being observed too
closely, we are probably facing an instinct, which is stronger in the
brute than in the man; but it is the fact, not the explanation thereof,
which is important. The simple fact is, that wild birds and beasts will
not endure watching; and you begin to sympathize with their notion when
you mark the eyes of a stalking cat, with their terrible fire just
before she springs. There is always more or less of that fire in a
watchful eye; you may see it glow or blaze under a man’s narrowed lids
before he takes quick action; and it is the kindling of that dangerous
light which a sensitive creature expects and avoids when he finds you
watching him.

Did you ever follow an old cock-partridge in the woods with intent to
kill him? If so, you have a living picture of the truth I have
explained theoretically. Near our towns the partridge (ruffed grouse)
is very wild, taking wing at your approach; but in the deep woods he is
almost fearless. Even when you stumble into a flock of the birds,
frightening them out of their calm, they are apt to flit into the trees
and remain absolutely motionless. They are then hard to find, so well
do they blend with their background; and if they are young birds, they
will hold still after you discover them. Since they were helpless
chicks they have trusted to quietness to conceal them; it serves them
very well, much better than running away from stronger enemies; and the
habit is strong upon them, as upon young ducks and other game-birds
before they have learned to trust their wings. But when you stumble
upon an old cock-grouse you meet a bird that has added experience to
instinct, and that knows when to move as well as when to sit still. He
dodges out of sight as you raise your rifle; as you follow him he
bursts away on whirring wings and slants up into a tree in a distant
part of the wood. Marking where he lights, you try to find him,
cat-footing around his perch, peering into every tree-top, putting a
“crik” in the back of your neck. For a half-hour, it may be, you search
for him in vain; suddenly there he is, and—b’r-r-r-r! he is gone. The
odd thing is that he sits still so long as you cannot find him; not a
feather stirs or a foot shifts or an eyelid blinks even when your
glance roves blindly over him; you may give him up and go away, leaving
him motionless; but the instant you see him he seems to know it, and in
that instant he is off. This is not a single or an accidental but a
typical experience; any woodsman who has hunted ruffed grouse with a
rifle will smile as he tells you, “That’s true; but I can’t explain
it.”

A third bit of woods lore, of which we shall presently make good use,
is that natural birds and animals have a lively interest in every new
or strange thing they meet. Far from being occupied in a constant
struggle for existence, as the books misinform us, their lives are full
of leisure; they have plentiful hours for rest or play or roving, and
in these idle times they get most of their fun out of life by indulging
their curiosity. I fancy that in this respect, also, most people are
still natural creatures, seeing that men or women in a crowd are as
easily set to stretching their necks as any flock of ducks or band of
caribou.

So strong is the animal’s inquisitive instinct (for it surely is an
instinct, the basis of all education, and without it we should be
fools, learning nothing) that he will readily give over his play or
even his feeding to investigate any new thing which catches his
attention. I speak now not of fearsome things, which may properly alarm
the wood folk, but of pretty or harmless or attractive things, such as
the repeated flash of a looking-glass or the rhythmic swing of a
handkerchief or a whistled tune, which commonly bring wild creatures
nearer with forward-set ears and eyes with questions in them. In a
word, so far as I have observed birds and beasts, their first or
natural attitude toward every new object, unless it be raising fearful
smells or moving toward them with hostile intent, is invariably one of
curiosity rather than of fear.

One proof of this universal trait, to me, is that when I approach wild
animals carelessly they often run away; but of the hundreds that have
approached me when I was quiet in the woods, every one without
exception showed plainly by his action that he was keen to find out who
or what I might be. Young animals are more inquisitive than old, having
everything to learn, and they are easily attracted; but age cannot
stale the wonder of the world for them, and I have never chanced to
meet an old doe, no, nor a tough old bull moose, that did not come near
to question me if the chance were given. Of the larger wood folk
Mooween the bear is perhaps the least inquisitive; yet once an old bear
came so close to me, his eyes a question and his nose an exclamation
point, that I could have touched him before his curiosity was
satisfied; and several times, when I have been watching the
berry-fields, a bear and her cubs have noticed some slight motion of
mine and have left their feast of blueberries to approach rather too
near for my comfort. At close quarters an old she-bear is a little
uncertain. Commonly she runs away in sudden panic; but should you get
between her and her cub, and the piggish little fellow squeal out as if
frightened or hurt, she may fly into a fury and become dangerous to a
man unarmed.

The obvious thing to do, in view of what has been learned, is to hold
physically and mentally still when you meet a wild animal, and so take
advantage of his curiosity. That is very easy when he happens to find
you at rest, for then he is bound to find out something about you
before he goes; but even when he catches you afoot you may still have a
fair chance if you stop in your tracks and move no muscle while he is
looking. Remember that so long as you are motionless you puzzle him;
that you should advance only when his head is turned away, and that you
should never move directly at any animal, but to one side, as if you
would give him plenty of room in passing. If you must change your
position or attitude while he is looking, move gently and very slowly,
avoiding every appearance of haste or nervousness. If he vanishes after
one keen look, be sure he is a veteran that has seen men before, and
bide still where you are. The chances are ten to one that no sooner
does he think himself hidden than he will turn to have another look at
you. It is always in your favor, since you have the better eyes, that
an animal has the habit of concealment, and so long as you pretend not
to see him he is very apt to think himself unseen.

Such a method applies particularly well to all members of the deer
family, with their insatiable curiosity; but it serves almost as well
with beasts of prey, which may be so surprised by meeting a motionless
man that they will often “point” him in a way to suggest a setter
pointing a woodcock. We think of the fox, for example, as the most
cunning of animals; like the dolls’ dressmaker in Our Mutual Friend, he
seems to be saying, “Oh, I know your tricks and your manners”; yet on a
good tracking-snow I have trailed many foxes to their day-beds, and
have found that with few exceptions they act in the same half-puzzled,
half-inquisitive way. And this is the fashion of it:

Looking far ahead on the dainty trail you suddenly catch a glimpse of
orange color, very warm against the cold whiteness of the snow, which
tells you where Eleemos the sly one, as Simmo calls him, is curled on a
warm rock or stump with the winter sunshine fair upon him. Then you
must leave the trail, as if you were not following it, and advance on
noiseless feet till the fox raises his head, when you must “freeze” in
your tracks. If he is a tramp fox (that is, one which has come hunting
here out of his own territory) or a veteran that has already seen too
much of men and their devices, he will dodge out of sight and be seen
no more; but if he is an ordinary young fox, especially a cub
weathering his first winter, he will almost certainly investigate that
odd motionless object which was not there when he went to sleep. After
“pointing” you a moment he slips into the nearest cover, not turning
his head in your direction, but watching you keenly out of the corners
of his yellow eyes. When he thinks himself hidden from your sight he
circles to get your wind; and on this side or that you will have two or
three good glimpses of him before he floats away—or seems to, so
lightly does he run—to hunt up another day-bed. Your last view of him
shows a slyly inquisitive little beast, perfectly self-possessed; but
as he disappears you notice a nervous, quivering, fluttering motion of
his great brush, which gives him away as a tail betrays a dog, and
which says that Eleemos is greatly excited or puzzled over something.

Better than roaming noisily through the woods in search of game is to
sit still and let the game come to you—an arrangement which puts you at
your ease, and at the same time encourages the animal to indulge his
curiosity without alarm. You may not see so many birds or beasts in
this way, but some of them you shall see much more intimately; and a
single inquisitive jay may teach you more of nature than all the bird
books in the world, as I have learned more of Latin humanity from
Angelo, who polishes my shoes, than from Gibbon’s Decline and Fall of
the Roman Empire. Very often, if you hold perfectly still, a wild
animal will pass down the runway close at hand without even seeing you,
and you must draw his attention by a chirp or a slight motion. Then,
when he whirls upon you in astonishment, his eyes saying that he was
never so surprised in his life, observe him casually as it were,
veiling your interest and never staring at him as if he were a wild or
strange beast, but greeting him rather as one you have long known.

At such a moment quietness is the best medicine—quietness and friendly
eyes. If the animal wavers, a low song or a whistled tune may or may
not be helpful; it depends entirely on the tune. You are to keep
physically quiet, because any sudden motion will alarm the sensitive
creature, so near is he to the unknown; and mentally quiet, because
excitement is as contagious as fear or measles, or any other disease of
mind or body. When I am alone in the woods wild animals are rarely hard
to approach, and when I am sitting quietly by a runway they show no
fear of me whatever, drawing near with questioning eyes or moving away
reluctantly; but when I take another with me, especially one who grows
excited in the presence of big game, the same animals appear
suspicious, uneasy, and end by bolting away as if we had frightened
them.

One day there came to my camp a friend who was eager to see a deer at
close range, but who was doubtful of my assurance that animals could
neither see nor smell him if he knew how to hold still. When I promised
him a deer at ten feet he jumped for his camera, saying that in such an
incredible event he would get what he had always wanted, a picture of
the graceful creature against a background of his native woods, in soft
light and shadow instead of the glaring black-and-white of a
flashlight. At that disturbing proposition all his doubts moved into
me, who have always found camera folk a fidgety folk. What with their
fussing and focusing and everlasting uneasiness over distance or time
or shutter, or something else which is never right and ready, they are
sure to bedevil any wild creature before he comes within speaking
distance; so I took my friend and his camera along without faith,
hoping for the best.

Our stand was a hardwood ridge where deer often passed on their way to
the lake, and we had been sitting there hardly an hour when I saw a
young spikebuck coming down the runway. The next moment there was a
gasping “Oh, there’s a deer!” from the man who had been warned to keep
mentally still. Then began the inevitable tinkering with the camera,
which had been thrice prepared and was still as unready as all its
kind. More than once I had sat in that precise spot while deer passed
at a distance of three or four yards without noticing me; but now the
little buck caught an uneasy motion and halted with head high and eyes
flashing. If ever there was a chance for a wonderful picture, he
offered it; but he did not like the focusing, or whatever it was, and
after endless delay the camera clicked on a white flag bobbing among
the shadows, where it looked in the negative like a smear of sunlight.



The camera reminds me of another way of approaching deer, a way often
followed by summer campers; namely, by chasing the swimming animal in a
canoe. I have but one word to say of such a method, and that is, Don’t!
When a deer is crossing broad water you can get as close to him as you
will; you can take a grip on him and let him tow your canoe, as
thoughtless people sometimes do, encouraged by their guides; but I
suggest that it would be much better to shoot the creature and have
done with it.

A deer’s powers are very delicately balanced; he is nervous, high
strung, easily upset. Even on land, where he can distance you in a
moment, he begins to worry if he finds you holding steadily to his
trail; and I have known a young deer to become so flustered after he
had been jumped and followed a few times that he began to act in most
erratic fashion, and was very easily approached. When you chase him in
the water, and he finds that he cannot get away from you, he may give
up and drown, as a rabbit submits without a struggle when a weasel
rises in front of him; but a vigorous deer is more apt to become highly
excited, to struggle wildly, to waste ten times as much energy as would
keep him afloat, to jump his heart action at a dangerous rate; and then
a very little more will finish him as surely as a bullet in the brain.

Twice have I seen deer thus killed by thoughtless campers, the last
victim being a splendid buck full grown. Two men saw him swimming an
arm of Moosehead Lake, and launched a canoe with no unkinder purpose
than to turn him back to shore, so that a party of sportsmen there
might get a picture of him. The buck labored mightily; but the paddles
were swift, and wherever he turned the danger appeared close in front
of him. Suddenly he rose in the water, pawing the air, and heaved over
on his side. When the canoe reached him he was dead; and the surprising
thing is that dissection revealed no ruptured blood-vessel nor any
other visible cause of his death. It was probably a matter of heart
paralysis. Such an ending was unusual, I know; but undoubtedly many of
these overwrought animals reach shore exhausted, spent to the limit,
and lie down in the first good cover, never to rise again.

Moose and caribou are stronger swimmers than deer, and of tougher
fiber; but it is still dangerous, I think, to chase them in the water.
Once I saw a canoe following close behind a cow and a calf moose, the
canoeists yelling wildly to hurry up the pace. Had they thought to look
once into the eyes of the struggling brutes, they might have learned
something which they ought to know. As the calf lagged farther and
farther behind, the mother turned to come between him and the canoe,
and remained there trying to urge and push the little fellow along. So
they reached shallow water at last, found their footing, and plunged
into the cover. The canoe turned away, and no doubt the incident was
soon forgotten. I never saw the canoemen again, but I saw one of the
moose. A few days later, in passing through the woods on that side of
the lake, I found the calf stretched out dead where he had fallen, not
fifty yards from the water’s edge.

Perhaps another “don’t” should here be memorized for the happy occasion
when you find a fawn or a little cub in the woods, and are moved most
kindly to pet him. If the mother is half-tame, or has lived near a
clearing long enough to lose distrust of the man-scent, it may do no
harm to treat her fawn or cub as you would a puppy; but to handle any
wild little creature is to do him an injury. Until a fawn is strong
enough to travel the rough country in which he was born, the doe often
leaves him hidden in the woods, where he lies so close and still that
you may pass without seeing him. Once you discover him, however, and he
knows that he is seen, his beautiful eyes begin to question you with a
great wonder. He has no fear of you whatever (this while he is very
young, or before he begins to follow his mother); he will sometimes
follow you when you go away, and he is such a lovable creature, so
innocent and so appealing, that it is hard to keep your hands from him.
Let him sniff your palm if he will, or lick it with his rough tongue
for the faint taste of salt; but as you value his life don’t pet him or
leave the scent of you on his delicate skin. A wild mother knows her
own by the sense of smell chiefly; if she finds the startling man-scent
where she expected a familiar odor, she becomes instantly alarmed, and
then the little one is a stranger to her or a source of violent anger.

Once, before I learned better than to handle any helpless cub, I saw a
doe drive her own fawn roughly away from her, out of my sight and
hearing. I had petted the fawn a little (a very little, I am glad to
remember) and looked with wonder on the mother’s anger, not
understanding it till some time later, when I learned of a similar
incident with a sadder ending. Not far from my camp a sportsman with
his guide found a fawn hidden near the stream where they were fishing,
and being completely won by the beautiful innocent, as most men are,
they petted him to their hearts’ content. When an old doe, the mother
presumably, appeared heading in their direction they thoughtfully
withdrew, hiding at a distance to watch the family reunion. The doe
seemed to hasten her steps when she saw that the fawn was on his feet,
instead of lying close where she had left him; but when near him she
suddenly stiffened, with the hair bristling on her neck. Two or three
times she thrust out her nose, only to back away, and once she raised
the harsh alarm-cry that a doe utters when she smells danger. Then, as
the little fellow trotted up to her on his wabbly legs, she leaped upon
him in fury and trampled him to death.








VIII

ON KEEPING STILL


To return to our first lesson, of quietude: it was impressed upon me
unconsciously, like most good lessons, before I had any thought that I
was learning the true way of the woods. The teacher was Nature herself
(she seldom fails to quiet boy or man if left alone with him), and the
school-room was a lonely berry-pasture surrounded by pine and hardwood
forests. The berry-pickers, a happy and carefree lot, often let me go
with them while I was yet too small to find my way among the tall
swamp-blueberry bushes, and would leave me under a tree at the edge of
the woods, with an armful of berry-laden branches to keep me busy while
they wandered far away in search of the best picking. Sitting there in
the breathing solitude, occupied with the task of filling my tin cup
with berries and well content with my lot (for the woods always had a
fascination for me, and seemed most friendly when I was alone), I would
presently “feel” that something was watching me. There was never any
suggestion of fear in the impression, only an awakening to the fact
that I was not alone, that some living thing was near me. Then, as I
looked up expectantly, I would almost always find a bird slipping
noiselessly through the branches overhead, or a beastie creeping
through the cover at my side; and in his bright eyes, his shy approach,
his withdrawal to appear in another spot, I read plainly enough that he
was asking who I was or what I was doing there. And by a whistled tune
or a drumming on my cup, or by flashing a sunbeam into his eyes from a
pocket glass, I always tried to hold him as long as I could.

This curious sense or feeling of being watched, by the way, is very
real in some men, who do not regard it as a matter of chance or
imagination. I have known of two elaborate courses of “laboratory”
experiments which aimed to determine how far such a feeling is
trustworthy, and both resulted in a neutral or fifty-fifty conclusion;
but I wonder, if the experiments had been tried on Indians or natural
men under natural conditions, whether the result might not have been
quite different. The fact that the first fifty men you meet get lost or
turned around in a trackless forest is significant for the fifty, and
for the vast majority of others; but it means nothing to the one
bushman who can go where he will without thought or possibility of
being lost, because of his sure sense of direction.

So, possibly, with this feeling of being watched: it may be too
intangible for experiment, or even for definition. Many times since
childhood when I have been alone in the big woods, fishing or holding
vigil by a wilderness lake, I have the feeling, at times vaguely and
again definitely, that strange eyes were upon me. Occasionally, it is
true, I have found nothing on looking around, either because no animal
was there or because he was too well hidden to be seen; but much more
often the feeling proved true to fact—so often, indeed, that I soon
came to trust it without doubt or question, as Simmo my Indian still
does, and a few other woodsmen I have known. It is possible that one’s
ears or nose may account for the feeling; that some faint sound or odor
may make itself felt so faintly that one has the impression of life
without knowing through what channel the impression is received. Of
that I am not at all sure; at the moment it seems that some extra sense
is at work, more subtle than smell or hearing; and, whether rightly or
wrongly, it is apparently associated with the penetrating stare of an
animal’s eyes on your back.

To quote but a single incident, out of several that come to my memory:
I was once sitting on the shore of a lake at twilight, wholly intent on
following the antics of a bull moose I had called into the open. He was
on the other side of a small bay, ranging up and down, listening,
threshing the bushes with his antlers, blowing his penny-trumpet at
intervals,—in a dozen impatient ways showing what a young and foolish
moose he was. A veteran would have kept to the cover till he had
located what he came for. I had ceased my bellowing when the bull first
answered, had been thrilled by his rush through the woods, had cheered
him silently when he burst into the open, grunting and challenging like
a champion; now I was quietly enjoying his bewilderment at not finding
the tantalizing cow he had just heard calling. He did not see or
suspect me; I had the comedy all to myself, and was keenly interested
to know how he would act when he rounded the bay, as he certainly
would, and found me sitting in his path. Because he was big and
truculent and a fool, I did not know what to expect; my canoe floated
ready against the outer end of a stranded log, where a push would send
it and me into deep water. I mention these details simply to show where
my thoughts were.

As I watched the play in the hushed twilight, suddenly came the feeling
that something was watching me. The bull had started around the bay in
my direction; possibly his eyes had picked me out—but no, he was in
plain sight, and the feeling is always associated with something
unseen. Without changing position I looked carefully all about,
searching the lake and especially the woods, which were already in deep
shadow. Finding no bird or beast, no motion, nothing alarming, I turned
to question the bull, who had halted to sound his ridiculous trumpet.
He was perhaps fifty or sixty yards away. He had not yet seen me; I had
no fear of him, no anxiety whatever; yet again came the feeling, this
time insistent, compelling, as if some one had touched me and said,
“Get away!” I did so promptly, jumping to my feet; and out of a fir
thicket behind me charged another bull that I had not dreamed of
calling.

By his size, his antlers, his fierce grunting, I recognized this brute
on the instant. I had met him before, once on a trail, once on the lake
shore, and had given him all the room he wanted. He was a grizzled old
bull, morose and ugly, that seemed to have lost his native fear of
man—from a galling wound, perhaps, or from living an outcast life by
himself. He was a little crazy, I judged. That he was dangerous I knew
from the fact that he had previously made an unprovoked attack upon my
Indian. He, too, had heard the call; had approached it from behind as
stealthily as a cat, and had no doubt watched me, puzzled by my
stillness, till my first decided motion brought him out on the jump.
But I am wandering away from the small boy getting his first lessons in
the woods, and learning that the important thing is to hold perfectly
still.

Later, when eight or nine years old, I went alone day after summer day
to the wild berry-pastures. When my big pail would hold no more, I
would make a bowl by bashing in the top of my hat, and fill it to the
brim with luscious blueberries. These with a generous slice of bread
made an excellent lunch, which I always ate within sight of a bird’s
nest, or the den of a fox, or some other abode of life that I had
discovered in the woods. And again, as I sat quiet in the solitude, the
birds and small animals might be led by curiosity to approach as
fearlessly as when I was too small to harm them. Now a vixen, finding
me too near her den and cubs, would squall at me impatiently, like a
little yellow dog with a cat’s voice; or again, a brooding bird that
objected to my scrutiny would first turn her tail to me, and presently
come round again, and finally get mad and flutter about my head,
scolding loudly to chivvy me away. So it often happened that one had
nearer or happier or more illuminating glimpses of wild life in that
small hour of rest than would be possible in a month of roaming the
woods with gun or collecting-box.

Once as I was eating my lunch under the pines, meanwhile watching a den
I had found to see what might come out of it, a crow sailed in on
noiseless wings and lit so near me that I hardly dared wink for fear he
would notice the motion. My first thought was that he was nest-robbing
(a crow is very discreet about that business), but he appeared rather
to be listening, cocking his head this way or that; and from a lazy
hawing in the distance I concluded he was satisfying himself that his
flock was occupied elsewhere and that he was quite alone. Presently he
hitched along the branch on which he stood and glided off to the crotch
of a pine-tree, where he began to uncover what was hidden under a mat
of brown needles. The first thing he took out was a piece of glass,
which sparkled with rainbow colors in a stray glint of sunshine. Then
came a bit of quartz with more sparkles, a shell, a silvery buckle, and
some other glistening objects which I could not make out. He turned his
treasures over and over, all the while croaking to himself in a pleased
kind of way; then he put them all back, covered them again with
needles, and slipped away without a sound. Having kept tame crows, I
knew that they are forever stealing and hiding whatever bright objects
they find about the house; and here in the pine woods was a thing to
indicate that wild crows, perhaps all of them, have the same covetous
habit.

Another day, a heavenly day when the budding woods were vocal and life
stirred joyously in every thicket, I took a jews’-harp from my pocket
and began to twang it idly. No, there was nothing premeditated in the
act. I had been roving widely, following the winds or the bird-calls
till a sunny opening invited me to rest, and had then fingered the
music-maker with no more purpose than the poet’s boy, who “whistled as
he went for want of thought.” The rhythmic, nasal twanging was a sound
never heard in that place before or since, I think, and the first to
come hurriedly to investigate was a bright-colored warbler, whose name
I did not know; nor did I care to know it, feeling sure that by some
note or sign he would presently suggest a name for himself, which would
please me better than the barbarous jargon I might find in a bird-book.
The alert little fellow lit on a branch within three feet of my face,
turning his head so as to view me with one eye or the other when I kept
quiet, or chirping his indignation when I twanged the jews’-harp. Next
came a jay, officious as the town constable; then more birds, half
concealing their curiosity under gentle manners; and a squirrel who had
no manners at all, scolding everybody and scurrying about in a fashion
which seemed dangerous to his excited head.

As I watched this little assembly, which seemed to be asking, “What’s
up? What’s up?” the meaning of it suddenly dawned on me like a
surprising discovery. When I entered the opening I knew simply that
birds or beasts would draw near if they found me quiet; before I left
it I had found the explanation: that all the wood folk are intensely
curious, as curious as so many human gossips, but without any of their
malice; that by inner compulsion they are drawn to any strange sight or
sound, as a crowd collects when a man cuts a caper or throws a fit or
raises a whoop or looks up into the air, or does anything else out of
the ordinary. When you appear in the quiet woods every bird or beast
within sight or hearing is agog to know about you; they are like the
Nantucket-Islanders, who named their one public hack the “Who’s Come?”
Because you are a stranger, and what you do is none of their business,
they are all the more interested in you and your doings; you come to
them with all the charm of the unknown, the unexpected; and they will
gratify their curiosity, fearlessly and most pleasantly, so long as you
know how to stimulate or play upon it and to hold still while enjoying
it.

All that is natural enough, as natural as life; but it is not written
in any book of natural history, and it came to me that day as a
wonderful discovery. It suggested at once the right way to study birds
or beasts, as living creatures; it has since led to many a fascinating
glimpse of the wood-folk comedy, and to a lifelong pleasure which is
too elusive to be set down in words. At the bottom of it, I suppose, is
the fact that in every wild or natural creature is something, at once
mysterious and familiar, which appeals powerfully to your interest or
sympathy, as if you saw a faint shadow of your other self, or caught a
fleeting memory of that vanished time when you lived in a child’s world
of wonder and delight.

From the beginning, therefore, I met all birds and animals in a child’s
impersonal way; which, strangely enough, ascribes personality to every
living thing, yes, and honors it. These inquisitive little rangers of
the wood or the berry-pasture, shy and exquisitely alert, were all
individuals like myself, each one seeking the joy of life in his own
happy way. My only regret was that I was too clumsy, too obtrusive, too
ignorant of the way of the wild, and so frightened many a timid bird or
beast that I would gladly have known.

All this, too, is perfectly natural; the instinctive attitude of a
child, as of an animal, is one of curiosity rather than of fear or
destruction. If left to his natural instincts, a child meets every
living creature with a mixture of shyness or timidity and bright
interest; he becomes an enemy of the wild, learning to frighten and
harry and kill, not from nature but from the evil example of his
elders. I could prove that beyond a peradventure, I think, if this were
the place; but there is no need of any man’s demonstration. Go yourself
to the big woods at twilight, leaving custom behind you; go alone and
unarmed; hear that rustle of leaves, that tread of soft feet which
brings you to an instant halt; see that strange beast which glides out
into the trail and turns to look at you with luminous eyes. Then
quickly examine your own mental state, and you will know the truth of a
man’s natural or instinctive attitude toward the mystery of life.

Unfortunately the wild birds and beasts near our home have learned that
man is unnatural, a creature to be feared, and their curiosity has
given place to another motive. The young still display their natural
bent freely; but the old have heard too many of our guns, have been too
often disturbed by our meddlesome dogs or worthless cats, have suffered
too much at the hands of outrageous egg-collectors or skin-collectors
to be any longer drawn to us when we go afield. As you go farther away
from civilization it becomes easier to play on the animals’ native
curiosity; in the far North or the remote jungle, or wherever man is
happily unknown, they still come fearlessly to investigate you, or to
stand quiet, like the ptarmigan, watching with innocent eyes as you
pass them by. In the intermediate regions, which are harried by
sportsmen for a brief period in the autumn and then left to a long
solitude, the animals are wild or tame according to season; and it has
seemed to me, not always but on occasions, that in some subtle way they
distinguish between man and man, taking alarm at the first sniff of a
hunter, but stopping to show their interest in a harmless woods-rover.

This last is a mere theory, to be sure, and to some it may appear a
fanciful one; but it rests, be assured, upon repeated experience. Thus,
I came once at evening to a camp of hunters who were in a sorry plight.
They were in a good deer country, and had counted largely on venison to
supply their table; but for more than a week they had tasted no meat,
and they were very hungry. The deer were wild as hawks, they assured
me. They had hunted every day; but because of the game’s wildness and
the dry weather, which made the leaves rustle loudly underfoot, it had
proved impossible to approach near enough for a shot—all of which made
me think that, if you want to see game, you should leave your gun at
home. I had met about a dozen deer that day; most of them were within
easy range, and a few of them stood with questioning eyes while a man
might have made ready his camera and taken a picture of them.

The very next morning, and within a mile of the hunters’ camp, I
witnessed a familiar but fascinating display of deer nature. At sunrise
I approached a bog, bordering a stream where a few good trout might be
found, and on the edge of the opening stood a doe and her well-grown
fawn, not twenty yards away. The fawn, a little buck with the nubs of
his first antlers showing, threw up his head as I appeared, and in the
same instant I dropped to the ground behind a mossy log. No whistle or
sound of alarm followed the action; so I scraped a mat of moss from the
log, put it on for a bonnet, and cautiously raised my head.

The old doe was still feeding; the buck stood like a living statue, his
whole attention fastened on the spot where I had disappeared. He had
seen something, he knew not what, and was waiting for it to show itself
again. When my bonnet appeared his eyes seemed to enlarge and flash as
he caught the motion. Without changing his footing, for his surprise
seemed to have rooted him in the ground, he began to sway his body to
left or right, stretching his head high or dropping it low, taking a
dozen graceful attitudes in order to view the queer bit of moss from
different angles. Then he slowly raised a fore foot, put it down very
gently, raised it again, stamped it down hard. Getting no response to
his challenge, he sidled over to his mother, still keeping his eyes
fastened on the log. At his touch or call she lifted her head, pointing
her nose straight at me, as if he had somehow told her where to give
heed.

It was a wonderful sight, the multicolored bog spread like a rug at the
feet of the glorious October woods, and standing on the crimson fringe
of it these two beautiful creatures, demanding with flashing eyes who
the intruder might be. For a full minute, while I held motionless, the
doe kept her eyes steadily on the log; then, “Nothing there, little
buck; don’t worry,” she said in her own silent way, and went to feeding
once more.

“But there is something; I saw it,” insisted the little buck, nudging
his mother by swinging his head against her side. That was the first
and only time, in that quick swing, when he took his eyes from what
attracted them. The doe looked a second time, saw nothing uncommon, and
had turned to feed along the edge of the opening when the little buck
recalled her in some way. “Can’t you see it, that white thing like a
face under the moss?” he was saying. “There! it moved again!”

The mother, whose back was turned to me, twisted her head around as if
to humor him, and to interest her I swayed the moss bonnet to and fro
like a pendulum. At that she whirled, surprise written large on her,
and I dropped my head, leaving her staring. When I looked again both
deer were coming nearer, the mother ahead, the fawn holding back as if
to say, “Careful now! It’s big, and it’s hiding just behind that log.”
So they drew on warily, stopping to stamp a fore foot, and every time
they challenged I gave the bonnet an answering wag. When they were so
near that I knew they must soon distinguish my eyes from the moss, I
sank out of sight. I was listening for their alarm-call or for the thud
of their flying feet when a gray muzzle slid over the log, and I laid
my hand fair on the mother’s cheek before she bounded away.

Now there was nothing strange or new in all that; on the contrary, it
was very much like what I had observed in other inquisitive deer. The
only surprising part of the comedy was that the doe, though she had
felt the touch of my hand and no doubt smelled the man behind it,
stopped short after a few jumps and turned to stare at the log again.
That she was still curious, still unsatisfied, was plain enough; what
puzzles me to know is, whether she would have acted in the same way if
one of the hungry hunters had been waiting in my shoes for the chance
or moment to kill her.








IX

AT CLOSE RANGE


It is easy, much easier than you think, to get close to wild birds and
beasts; for after you have met them a few times in the friendly,
impersonal way I have tried to describe, two interesting traits appear:
the first, that they do not see you clearly so long as you hold still;
the second, that even their keen noses lose track of you after you have
been quiet for a little time.

The eye is a weak point in all animals I have chanced to observe, which
apparently depend less on sight than on any other sense—so far as
safety goes, that is. In gratifying their curiosity they seem to be all
eyes. At other times they will catch an abrupt or unusual motion
quickly enough; but they are strangely blind to any motionless object
however large or small. Repeatedly when I have been sitting quiet,
without concealment but with “neutral” clothes that harmonize with the
soft woods colors, I have known deer, moose, caribou, bear, wolf, fox,
lynx, otter, alert beasts of every kind, to approach within a few
yards, giving no heed till a chirp or a slight motion called their
attention. Then they would whirl upon me in astonishment, telling me by
their attitude that till then they had not noticed or suspected me.
Almost invariably at such times animals of the deer family would come a
step nearer, their heads high, their eyes asking questions; but beasts
of prey after one keen look would commonly drop their heads, as if I
were of no consequence, and slyly circle me to get my wind. In either
event success or a better view of the animal depended on just one
condition, which was to hold absolutely still. So long as I met that
condition, none of these wary beasts seemed to have any clear notion
what they were looking at.

Occasionally, indeed, their lack of discernment almost passes belief.
One winter day, while crossing a frozen lake in Ontario, I noticed a
distant speck moving on the snow, and stopped in my tracks to watch it.
The speck turned my way, drew near and changed into an otter, who came
rollicking along in his merry way, taking one or two quick jumps on his
abbreviated legs and a long slide on his ample belly. As the air was
dry and very still I had no fear of his nose, which is not as sensitive
as many others (perhaps because of the peculiar valve or flap which
closes it tight when an otter swims under water); but his eyes and
other senses are extraordinarily good, and it seemed impossible that he
should overlook a man standing erect on the snowy ice, as conspicuous
as a fly in the milk. So I watched the approach with lively interest,
wondering how Keeonekh would act in comparison with other members of
his weasel family when he found himself near me, whether he would dart
away like a fisher, or ignore me like a mink, or show his teeth at me
like a little stoat.

On he came, confidently, as an otter travels, giving no heed to the
enemy in his path, till he halted with a paw resting on one of my
snow-shoes and began to wiggle his broad muzzle, as if he found
something in the air which he did not like. For several moments he
hesitated, sniffing here, listening there, looking sharply about the
lake, into the near-by woods, everywhere except up into my face, and
then went on as he had been heading, leaving a straight trail behind
him. No man can tell what was in his head, and a very intelligent head
it is; but his action seemed to say that he did not see me when he
passed literally under my nose. To him I was merely a stump, one of a
dozen that projected here or there above the ice near the shore.

Such an incident would be merely freakish if it happened once; but it
happens again and again, becoming almost common or typical, when a man
stands motionless in the presence of other birds or beasts. Twice in
the big woods has the Canada lynx, a dull beast in comparison with the
otter, passed me with an unseeing stare in his wild eyes. And I have
crouched in the snow on a treeless barren while a band of caribou filed
past, so near that I could see the muscles ripple under their sleek
skins and hear the click-click of their hoofs as they walked. The
greater part of the herd did not even notice me; the rest threw a
passing glance in my direction, one halting as if he had a moment’s
doubt, and went on without a sign of recognition.

The eyes of birds are keener, as a rule; but it is still a question
with me how much or how little they see of what is plain as a frog on a
log to human vision. An owl has excellent eyes, which are at their best
in the soft twilight; yet once as I sat quiet in the dusk a horned-owl
swooped and struck at a motion of my head, not seeing the rest of me,
one must think, since he is quick to take alarm when a man appears.
Another owl showed even less discernment in that he overlooked me, head
and all, at a yard’s distance and tried to get his game out from under
my feet.

A little dog had followed me that day, keeping out of sight till we
were far from home, when he showed himself in a waggish way, as if he
knew I would not have the heart to tie him up in that lonely place, as
he deserved. All day long he had a vociferous and a “bully” time making
a nuisance of himself, stirring up a hornets’ nest in every peaceful
spot, chasing deer out of sight with a blithe rowdydow, swimming out to
the raft on which I was fly-fishing, jumping in to get tangled in my
landing-net when I reached for a big trout—in twenty ways showing that
his business was to take care of me, though he was no dog of mine.

Late in the afternoon, as I rested beside the homeward trail, the
little dog rambled off by himself, still looking for trouble, like all
his breed. Presently there was a yelp, a scurry, a glimpse of broad
wings swooping, and back came the trouble-seeker like a streak, his
eyes saying, “Look at this thing I brought you!” and his ears flapping
like a pair of wings to help him along. Over him hovered a big
barred-owl, grim as fate, striking, missing, mounting, swooping again,
brushing me with his wings as he whirled around my head. Between my
heels and the log on which I was sitting my protector wedged himself
securely; the owl with a vicious snapping of his beak sailed up into an
evergreen and made himself invisible. In a moment or two the little dog
came out and was wagging his tail mightily over the adventure when the
owl slanted down on noiseless wings and struck a double set of claws
into him. Then I interfered, rising to my feet; and then, for the first
time I think, the owl saw me as something other than a stump and
vanished quickly in the spruce woods.

Hawks likewise have marvelous eyes for all things that move; but I
began to question the quality of their vision one day when I was
watching a deer, and a red-shouldered hawk lit so near me that I
reached out a hand and caught him.

Another afternoon I came upon a goshawk, keenest of all the falcons,
that had just killed a grouse in the tote-road I was following. He
darted away as I came round a bend; but thinking he might soon return,
for he is a bold and a persistent kind of pirate, I entered the woods
at a swift walk, as if going away. Then I worked cautiously back to the
road through a thicket, and waited on a log in deep shadow, some fifty
yards above where the grouse lay undisturbed. Luckily I had a rifle, an
accurate little twenty-two, often carried as medicine for “vermin,” and
I intended to kill the goshawk at the first chance. He is the viking
among birds, and as such has a romantic interest; but wherever he
appears he is a veritable pest, the most destructive of the hungry
hordes that come down from the North to play havoc with our game.

For a long time the goshawk hovered about, sweeping on tireless wings
high above the trees; but though I was quiet enough to deceive any bird
or beast, he held warily aloof. Once he disappeared, remaining so long
away that I was beginning to think he had wearied of the game of
patience, when I heard an eery call and saw him wheeling over the road
again. His absence became clear a little later when he perched on a
blasted pine, far out of range, where he remained watching for fifteen
or twenty minutes, his only motion being an occasional turning of the
head. That he was hungry and bound to have his own was plain enough;
the puzzle was why he did not come and get it, for it seemed highly
improbable that he would notice a motionless figure at that distance. I
think now that he was sailing high over my head when I re-entered the
trail; that he knew where I was all the time, not because he saw me on
the log, but because he did not see me go away. Before sunset he headed
off toward the mountain, going early to roost with all his kind.

Dawn found me back on the old road, as much to test the matter of
vision as to put an end to the game-destroyer. As a precaution I had
changed clothes, and now shifted position, avoiding the log because the
wary bird would surely take a look at it before coming down. My stand
was a weathered stump beside the road, against which I sat on a carpet
of moss, without concealment of any kind. At a short distance lay the
grouse, a poor crumpled thing, just as he had wilted under the hawk’s
swoop.

Thus a half-hour or more passed comfortably. A gorgeous cock-partridge
sauntered into the open, saw me when I nodded to him, and went slowly
off with that graceful, balancing motion which a grouse affects when he
is well satisfied with himself. Then, as the sun rose, there was a
swift-moving shadow, a rustle of pinions; the goshawk swept down the
road in front of me and lit beside his game. He was a handsome bandit,
in full adult plumage; his gray breast was penciled in shadowy lines,
while his back was a foggy blue, as if in his northern home he had
caught the sheen of the heavens above and of rippling waters beneath
his flight. His folded wings stood out squarely from his shoulders with
an impression of power, like an eagle’s. There was something noble in
his poise, in his challenging eye, in the forward thrust of his fierce
head; but the spell was broken at the first step. He moved awkwardly,
unwillingly it seemed; his great curving talons interfered with his
footing when he touched the earth.

This time, instead of a rifle, there was a trim shotgun across my
knees. The hawk was mine whether he stood quiet or leaped into swift
flight, and feeling sure of him now I watched awhile, wondering whether
he would break up his game with his claws, as some owls do, or tear it
to pieces with his hooked beak. For a moment he did neither, but stood
splendidly alert over his kill. Once he turned his head completely
around over either shoulder, sweeping his piercing glance over me, but
seeing nothing unusual. Then he seized his game in one foot and struck
his beak into the breast, making the feathers fly as he laid the
delicate flesh open. When I found myself weakening, growing sentimental
at the thought that it was his last meal, his last taste of freedom and
the wild, I remembered the grouse and got quietly on my feet. Though
busy with his feast, he caught the first shadow of a motion; I can
still see the gleam in his wild eyes as he sprang aloft.

I thought him beyond all harm as he lay on his back, one outstretched
wing among the feathers of his victim; but he struck like a flash when
I reached down for him carelessly. “Take that! and that! and remember
me!” he said, driving his weapons up with astonishing force, a force
that kills or paralyzes his game at the first grip. Four of his
needle-pointed talons went to the bone, and the others were well buried
in the flesh of my arm. The old viking had been some time with his
ancestors before I pried him loose.

As for the sense of smell, on which most animals depend for accurate
information, I have tried numerous experiments with deer, moose, bear
and other creatures to learn how far they can wind a man, and how their
powers compare one with another. There is no definite answer to the
problem, so baffling are the conditions of observing these shy beasts;
but you are in for some surprises, at least, when you attempt to solve
it in the open. You will learn, for example, that when a gale is
blowing the animals are more at sea than in a dead calm; or that in a
gusty wind you can approach them about as easily from one side as from
another. Such a wind rolls and eddies violently, rebounding from every
hill or point or shore in such erratic fashion that the animals have no
means of locating a danger when they catch a fleeting sniff of it. It
is for this reason, undoubtedly, that all game is uncommonly wild on a
windy day: the constant motion of leaves or tossing boughs breeds
confusion in their eyes, and the woodsy smells are so broken by
cross-currents that they cannot be traced to their source. So it has
happened more than once on a gusty day that a deer, catching my scent
on the rebound, has whirled and rushed straight at me, producing the
momentary illusion that he was charging.

With a steady but not strong wind blowing in their direction, I have
seen deer become alarmed while I was yet a quarter-mile away; this on a
lake, where there was nothing to interfere with the breeze or the
scent. On the burnt lands or the open barrens I have seen bear and
caribou throw up their heads and break away while I was even farther
removed. In a light breeze the distance is much shorter, varying from
fifty to two hundred yards, according to the amount of moisture in the
air. On days that are still or very dry, or when the air is filled with
smoke from a forest fire (the latter soon inflames all sensitive
nostrils), the animals are at sea again, and depend less on their noses
than on their eyes or ears.

Another surprising thing is, that the animal’s ability to detect you
through his sense of smell is largely governed by your own activity or
bodily condition. Thus, when a man is perspiring freely or moving
quickly, his scent is stronger and travels much wider than when he is
sauntering about. But if a man sits absolutely quiet, a clean man
especially, no animal can detect him beyond a few feet, I think, for
the reason that a resting man is like a resting bird or beast in that
he gives off very little body scent, which remains on the ground close
about him instead of floating off on the air currents. Even when the
trees are tossing in a gale there is little stir on the ground, not in
the woods at least, and the closer you hold to Mother Earth the less
likelihood is there of any beast smelling you.

All ground-nesting birds depend for their lives on this curious
provision of nature. Were it not for the fact that practically no scent
escapes while they are brooding their eggs, very few of them would live
to bring forth a family in a wood nightly traversed by such keen-nosed
enemies as the fox and the weasel. My old setter would wind a running
grouse or quail at an incredible distance, and would follow him by
picking his scent from the air; but I have taken that same dog on a
leash near the same birds when they were brooding their eggs, and he
could not or would not detect them unless he were brought within a few
feet, or (a rare occurrence) unless a creeping ground-breeze blew
directly from the nest into his face.

The same provision guards animals, such as deer and caribou, which
build no dens but leave their helpless young on the ground. Two or
three times, after finding a fawn in the woods, I have tested his
concealment by means of my young dog’s nose; and I may add that Rab
will point a deer as stanchly as he points a grouse or woodcock, for he
is still in the happy, irresponsible stage when everything that lives
in the woods is game to him. So long as the fawn remains motionless
where his mother hid him, the dog must be almost on top of him before
pointing or showing any sign of game. But if the little fellow runs or
even rises to his feet at our approach (fawns are apt to do this as
they grow older), the dog seems to catch the scent after the first
motion; he begins to cat-foot, his nose up as in following an air
trail, and steadies to a point while he is still many yards away from
where the fawn was hiding.

The nose of a wolf is keener than that of any dog I ever knew; yet I
once trailed a pack of wolves that passed within sixteen measured feet
of where two deer were sleeping in a hole in the snow. The wolves were
hunting, too, for they killed and partially ate a buck a little farther
on; but the trail said that they had passed close to these sleeping
deer without detecting them.

As for the man-scent, you may judge of that by the violent start or the
headlong rush when an animal catches the first alarming whiff of it. If
he passes quietly on his way, therefore, you may be reasonably sure he
has not smelled you. To the latter conclusion I have been forced many
times when I have been watching in the woods, sitting quiet for hours
at a stretch, and a deer or bear or fox, or some other beast with nose
as keen as a brier, has passed at a dozen yards’ distance without a
sign to indicate that he was aware of me. Some of these animals came
much nearer; so near, in fact, that I was scary of a closer approach
until I had called their attention to what lay ahead of them.

So long as you are seen or suspected, you need have little fear of any
wild beast (only the tame or half-tame are dangerous), but a brute that
stumbles upon you in an unexpected place or moment is always a problem.
Nine times out of ten he will fall all over himself in his haste to get
away; but the tenth time he may fall upon you and give you a mauling.
Moose, for example, are apt to strike a terrible blow with their fore
feet, or to upset a canoe when the jack-light approaches them; not to
attack, I think, at least not consciously, but in blind panic or to
ward off a fancied enemy. So when I have watched from the shore of a
lake and a moose came swinging along without noticing me, I have risen
to my feet or thrown my hat at the big brute when he was as near as I
cared to have him. And more than once, after a tremendous start of
surprise, he has come nearer with his hackles up as soon as he got over
the first effect of my demonstration. Yet when I am roaming the woods
that same brute will catch my scent at from two to five hundred yards,
and rush away before I can get even a glimpse of him.

That the same surprising sense-limitation is upon deer and other game
animals may be inferred from the following experience, which is typical
of many others. I was perched among some cedar roots on the shore of a
pond, one September day, watching a buck with the largest antlers I
have ever seen on one of his kind. I had been some time quiet when he
glided out to feed in a little bay, on my right; and my heart was with
him in the wish that he might keep his noble crown through the hunting
season, for his own pleasure and the adornment of the woods and the
confusion of all head-hunters. There was no breeze; but a moistened
finger told of a faint drift of air from the lake to the woods.

As I watched the buck, there came to my ears a crunching of gravel from
the opposite direction, and two deer appeared on the point at my left,
heading briskly down into the bay. They passed between my outstretched
feet and the water’s edge, where the strip of shore was perhaps three
yards wide; then they turned in my direction, seeing or smelling
nothing, went slowly up the bank and halted at the edge of the woods to
the right and a little behind me, so close that I dared not move even
my eyes to follow them. I measured the distance afterward, and found
that from their hoof-marks to the cedar root against which I rested was
less than eight feet. Imperceptibly I turned for another look, and saw
both deer at attention, their heads luckily pointed away from me. They
were regarding the big buck intently, as if to question him. They
showed no alarm as yet; but they were plainly uneasy, searching the
forest on all sides and at times turning to look over my head upon the
breathless lake. Every nervous action said that they found something
wrong in the air, some hint or taint or warning which they could not
define. So they moved alertly into the woods, halting, listening,
testing the air, using all their senses to locate a danger which they
had passed and left behind them.

From such experiences one might reasonably conclude that, like the
brooding grouse or the hidden fawn, a motionless man gives off so
little scent that the keenest nose is at fault until it comes almost
within touching distance. If any further proof is needed, you may find
it when you sleep in the open, and shy creatures draw near without any
fear of you. By daylight deer, bear and moose are extremely timid; they
rarely come within eyeshot of your camp, and they vanish at the first
sniff which tells them that you have invaded their feeding-grounds. But
when you are well asleep the same animals will pass boldly through your
camp-yard; or they will awaken you, as they have many times awakened
me, when you are tenting or sleeping under the stars by some outlying
pond. If you lie quiet, content to listen, the invading animal will
move freely here or there without concern; but no sooner do you begin
to stir, however quietly, than he catches the warning scent, and a
thudding of earth or a smashing of brush tells the rest of the story.

I recall one night, cloudy and very still, when I slept under my canoe
on a strip of sand beside a wilderness lake. The movement of an animal
near at hand awoke me. In the black darkness I could see nothing; but
somehow I knew he was big, and aside from the crepitation of the sand,
which I plainly heard, I seemed to feel the brute near me. For a moment
there was a pause, a dead silence; then came a thump, a rattlety-bang;
the canoe shook as something hit the lower end of it, and the creature
moved away. There was nothing to be done without eyes, so I snuggled
the blanket closer and went to sleep again. In the morning there were
the tracks of a moose, a bull as I judged from the shape of his feet,
to say that he had come down the shore at a fast walk, halted, stepped
over the stern of the canoe, and went on without hastening his pace.

That was odd enough; but more surprising were some tracks on the other
side, between the bow of the canoe and the woods. Very faint and dainty
tracks they were, as if a soft pad had touched the sand here and there
in an uneven line; but they told of a fox who had come trotting along
under the bank, and who had passed in the night without awakening me.
That neither he nor the moose had smelled the sleeping man, or nothing
alarming in him at least, is about as near to certainty as you will
come in interpreting animal action.



There is another and not wholly unreasonable hypothesis which may help
to explain such phenomena; namely, that it is not the scent of man but
of excitement, anger, blood-lust or some other abnormal quality which
alarms a wild animal. It sounds queer, I know, to say that anger can be
smelled; but it is more than probable that anger or fierce excitement
of any kind distils in the body a kind of poison which is physical and
sensible. Such excitement certainly weakens a man, clogging his system
with the ashes of its hot fires; and there is no reason why it should
not smell to earth as well as to high heaven.

You have but to open your eyes and expand your nostrils for some
evidence of this matter. Bees when angered give off a pungent odor,
which is so different from the ordinary smell of the hive that even
your dull nose may detect the change of temper. The same is true of
even cold-blooded reptiles. When you find a rattler or a black-snake
squirming in the sun, you can smell him faintly at a few yards’
distance. Now stir him up with a pole, or pin him to the earth by
pressing a forked stick with short prongs over his neck. As the snake
becomes enraged he pours off a rank odor, very different from the musky
smell that first attracted your notice, and it travels much wider, and
clings to your clothes for an hour afterward. It is not only possible
but very likely, therefore, that strong emotions affect the bodies of
all creatures in a way perceptible to senses other than sight. If so,
one man who is peaceable and another who is angry or highly excited may
give off such different odors that a brute with sensitive nostrils may
be merely curious about the one and properly afraid of the other.

That wild animals instinctively fear the scent of humanity, as such, is
probably not true. The notion arises, I think, from judging the natural
animal by those we have made unnatural by abuse or persecution.
Whenever man penetrates a wild region for the first time he finds, as a
rule, that the animals have little fear of him, the tameness of wild
game having been noted with surprise by almost every explorer. It has
been noted also, but without surprise, by saints and ascetics who “for
the greater glory of God” have adopted a life of solitude and
meditation, and who have often found the birds or beasts about their
hermitage to be quite fearless of them, and receptive of their
kindness. Not till the abundant flocks and herds of a new region have
been harried and decimated by senseless slaughter do the survivors
begin to be fearful and unapproachable, as we unfortunately know them.
Yet even now, no sooner do we drop our persecution and assume a
rational or humane attitude than the wild ducks come to the boat
landing of a winter hotel, deer feed at our haystacks, and bears come
in broad daylight to comfort themselves at our garbage-cans. Such
things could hardly be if the fear of man were an age-old or
instinctive inheritance.

Nearer home, on any farm bordering the wilderness, you may see wild
deer feeding quite tamely about the edges of the cleared fields all
summer. I recall one such farm in Maine, where the owner had fifteen
acres of green oats waving over virgin soil—a glorious crop for me, but
for him an occasion of lamentation. You could go through that field at
any hour before six in the morning or after six at night and find a
dozen deer with a moose or two making themselves at home. The owner’s
cattle were kept out by a rail fence; but the moose simply leaned
against the fence and went through, while the nimble deer sailed over
the obstruction like grasshoppers. On all such farms the deer have the
scent of man almost constantly in their nostrils, and they are simply
watchful, running when you approach too near, but turning after a short
flight to have a look at you. At times you may see them feeding when
the scent of laborers or fishermen blows fairly over them. But when
October comes, and the law is “off,” and wild-eyed hunters appear with
guns in their hands and death in their thoughts, then the same deer
quickly become as other and wilder creatures, rushing off in alarm at
the first sniff of an enemy. The fact and the changed action are
evident enough; the only interesting question is, To what extent does
the smell of man change when he changes his peaceable ways?

Two or three times I have had opportunity to test the effect of the
human scent in another way, the first time being when I had the good
luck to see a natural child and a natural animal together. The child, a
baby girl just beginning to toddle, was making a journey by means of a
comfortable Indian paukee on my back, and I had left her in an opening
beside a portage trail while I went back to my canoe for a thing I had
forgotten. While I was gone, three deer sauntered into the opening.
They saw the baby, and were instantly as curious about her as so many
gossips, a little spotted fawn especially. The baby saw them, and began
creeping eagerly forward, calling or “crowing” as she went. The deer
saw and heard and smelled her every moment; yet they walked around her
with springy steps, now on this side, now on that, showing a world of
curiosity in their bright eyes, but never a sign of fear.

From a distance I watched the lovely scene, kindling at the beauty of
it, or feeling a bit anxious when I saw the sharp feet of the old doe a
little too near the sunny head or the outstretched hands. Then an eddy
of wind from the mountain got behind me and whirled over the deer. They
caught the scent and were away with a wild alarm-call, their white
flags flying, and the baby waving by-by as they vanished in the woods.

Quite naturally, therefore, when a sensitive animal runs away from me,
I find myself thinking that perhaps it is not the smell of humanity but
of some evil trait or quality which frightens him. I first laid down
this hypothesis after meeting a strange, childlike man, who had a
passion for roaming by himself in the fields or woods. White men, after
a puzzling acquaintance, would tap their heads or call him crazy; an
Indian would look once in his eyes and say, very softly, “The Great
Spirit has touched him.” He was all gentleness, without a thought or
possibility of harm in his nature. He was also without fear, and
perhaps for this reason he inspired no fear in others. When he appeared
in the woods, singing to himself, the animals would watch him for a
moment, and then go their ways quietly, as if they understood him. What
would happen if a race of such men lived near the wood folk must be
left to the imagination.








MY POND: A SYMPHONY OF THE WOODS


X

THE TRAIL


To reach my pond you must leave your canoe on the shore of
Sungeegamook, the home lake, and go eastward through the big woods.
Yonder is the landing, that bank of green topped by “everlasting” and
blue asters, with a cleft like an arched doorway in the forest behind
it. A rugged jack-pine leans out over a bit of shingle, as if to
indicate a good place to beach your canoe, and there is something
curiously alive, almost sentient, in its attitude. The old tree seems
to watch your approach; through its leaves runs a low murmur of welcome
as you step ashore.

Entering the woods (and because you are alone, and therefore natural,
something in their dim aisles, their mysterious depths, their breathing
silence, makes you go gently) you find yourself in an old logging-road,
once a garish symbol of man’s destructiveness, but growing yearly more
subdued, more beautiful, since Nature began her work of healing. The
earth beneath your feet, the restful earth which the lumbermen left
torn by iron tools or rent by dynamite, has again put on her
soft-colored garments. Feathery beds of fern push boldly into the road
from shadowy places; wild grasses fill all its sunny openings with
their bloom and fragrance; and winding down through shade or sunshine
comes a trail made by the feet of deer and moose. Already these timid
animals have adopted the forgotten road as a runway; you may meet them
here when you return in the evening twilight.

Everywhere beside the trail are old marks of the destroyer. Noble
maples or cedars that were centuries growing have been slashed down,
dismembered, thrust aside to decay, and all because they stood in the
way of a lumber-boss who thought only of getting his cut of spruce down
to the lake. To look upon such trees, dead and shorn of their beauty,
is to feel pity or indignation; but Nature does not share your feeling,
being too abundant of life and resource to waste any moment in regret.
Already she is upbuilding what man has torn down. Glaring ax-wounds
have all disappeared under bandages of living moss; every fallen log
has hidden its loss under a mantle of lichen, soft and gray, which
speaks not of death but of life renewed.

Where the sun touches these prostrate giants a blush of delicate color
spreads over them. See, it deepens as you look upon it curiously, and
you examine it to find a multitude of “fairy-cups” on slender stems,
each lifting its scarlet chalice to the light. Very soft and inviting
seats they offer, yielding to your weight, sending up an odor as of
crushed herbs; but do not accept the invitation. If you must halt to
rest or to enjoy the stillness, sit not down on one of these mossy
logs, but before it at a little distance, and let its blended colors be
to your eye what the wind in the pine is to your ear, or the smell of
hemlock to your nostrils. Then will all your senses delight in harmony,
their natural birthright, while you rest by the way.

Where the old road winds about the end of a ridge, avoiding every steep
pitch, young balsams are crowding thickly into it; where it turns
downward to the lowlands, quick-growing alders claim it as their own;
and as you leave the lake far behind it begins to divide interminably,
each branch breaking into smaller branches, like the twigs of a tree as
you trace them outward. The twig ends with a bud in clear space; but
the farther or landward end of a logging-road dwindles to a deer-path,
the path to a rabbit-run, and the run vanishes in some gloomy cedar
swamp or trackless thicket where is no outlook on any side.

It is in such places, while you puzzle over another man’s road instead
of keeping your own trail straight, that you are most apt to get lost.
Coming back you need have no fear of going astray, since all these
trails lead to the main road, and thence downhill to the lake; but
going forward it is well to steer clear of all branch roads, which lead
nowhere and confuse the sense of direction.

Leaving the road behind, therefore, and heading still eastward, you
cross a ridge where the hardwoods stand, as their ancestors stood,
untouched by the tools of men. Immense trunks of beech or sugar-maple
or yellow birch tower upward wide apart, the moss of centuries upon
them; far overhead is a delicate tracery of leaves, a dance of light
against the blue, and over all is the blessed silence.

Beyond the ridge the ground slopes downward to a uniform level. Soon
the moss grows deeper underfoot, with a coolness that speaks of
perpetual moisture. The forest becomes dense, almost bewildering; here
a “black growth” of spruce or fir, there a tangle of moosewood, yonder
a swale where impenetrable alder-thickets make it impossible to hold a
straight course. Because all this growth is useless to the lumberman,
there is no cutting to be seen; but because I have passed this way
before, instinctively following the same course like an animal, a faint
winding trail begins to appear, with a bent twig or a blazed tree at
every turn to give direction.

As you move forward more confidently, learning the woodsman’s way of
looking far ahead to pick up the guiding signs before you come to them,
the dim forest suddenly brightens; a wave of light runs in, saying as
it passes overhead that you are near an opening. As if to confirm the
message, the trail runs into a well-worn deer-path, which looks as if
the animals that used it knew well where they were going. Clumps of
delicate young larches spring up ahead; between them open filmy vistas,
like windows draped in lace, and across one vista stretches a ribbon of
silver. A few more steps and—there! my little pond is smiling at you,
reflecting the blue deeps of heaven or the white of passing clouds from
its setting of pale-green larch-trees and crimson mosses.

And now, if you are responsive, you shall have a new impression of this
old world, the wonderful impression which a wilderness lake gives at
the moment of discovery, but never again afterward. As you emerge from
cover of the woods, the pond seems to awaken like a sleeper. See, it
returns your gaze, and on its quiet face is a look of surprise that you
are here. Enjoy that first awakening look; for there is more of wisdom
and pleasure in it, believe me, than in hurrying forth blindly intent
on making a map or catching a trout, or doing something else that calls
for sight to the neglect of insight. All sciences, including
chartography and angling, can easily be learned by any man; but
understanding is a gift of God, and it comes only to those who keep
their hearts open.

Your own nature is here your best guide, and it shows you a surprising
thing: that your old habitual impressions of the world have suddenly
become novel and strange, as if this smiling landscape were but just
created, and you were the first to look with seeing eyes upon the glory
of it. It tells you, further, if you listen to its voice, that creation
is all like this, under necessity to be beautiful, and that the beauty
is still as delightful as when the evening and the morning were the
first day. This dance of water, this rain of light, this shimmer of
air, this upspringing of trees, this blue heaven bending over all—no
artist ever painted such things; no poet ever sang or could sing them.
Like a mother’s infinite tenderness, they await your appreciation, your
silence, your love; but they hide from your description in words or
pigments.

Finally, in the lowest of whispers, your nature tells you that the most
impressive and still most natural thing in this quiet scene is the
conscious life that broods silently over it. As the little pond seems
to awaken, to be alive and sentient, so also does that noble tree
yonder when you view it for the first time, or that delicate orchid
wafting its fragrance over the lonely bog. Each reflects something
greater than itself, and it is that greater “something” which appeals
to you when you enter the solitude. Your impressions here are those of
the first man, a man who found many beautiful things in a garden, and
God walking among them in the cool of the day. Call the brooding life
God or the Infinite or the Unknown or the Great Spirit or the Great
Mystery—what you will; the simple fact is that you have an impression
of a living Being, who first speaks to you in terms of personality that
you understand.

So much, and more, of eternal understanding you may have if you but
tarry a moment under these larches with an open mind. Then, when you
have honored your first impression, which will abide with you always,
you may trace out the physical features of my pond at leisure. Just
here it is not very wide; your eye easily overlooks it to rest with
pleasure on a great mound of moss, colored as no garden of flowers was
ever colored, swelling above the bog on the farther shore. On either
hand the water sparkles wider away, disappearing around a bend with an
invitation to come and see. To the left it ends in velvety shadow under
a bank of evergreen; to the right it seems to merge into the level
shore, where shadow melts with substance in a belt of blended colors. A
few yards back from the shore groups of young larches lift their
misty-green foliage above the caribou moss; they seem not to be rooted
deep in the earth, but to be all standing on tiptoe, as if to look over
the brim of my pond and see their own reflections. Everywhere between
these larch groups are shadowy corridors; and in one of them your eye
is caught by a spot of bright orange. The spot moves, disappears,
flashes out again from the misty green, and a deer steps forth to
complete the wilderness picture with the grace of life.

Such is my pond, hidden away in the heart of a caribou bog, which is
itself well hidden in dense forest. Before I found it the wild ducks
had made it a summer home from time immemorial; and now, since I
disturb it no more, it is possessed in peace by a family of beavers;
yet I still think of it as mine, not by grace of any artificial law or
deed, but by the more ancient right of possession and enjoyment. A
hundred lakes by which I have tented are greater or more splendid; but
the first charm of any wilderness scene is its solitude, and on these
greater lakes the impression of solitude may be broken by the flash of
a paddle-blade in the sun, or the chuck of an ax under the twilight, or
the gleam of a camp-fire through the darkness. But here on my pond you
may know how Adam felt when he looked abroad: no raft has ever ruffled
its surface; no ax-stroke or moan of smitten tree has ever disturbed
its quiet; no camp-fire has ever gleamed on its waters. Its solitude is
still that of the first day; and it has no name, save for the Indian
word that came unbidden at the moment of finding it, like another
Sleeping Beauty, in the woods.

Do you ask how I came to find my pond? Not by searching, but rather by
the odd chance of being myself lost. I had gone astray one afternoon,
and was pushing through some black growth when an alarm rose near at
hand. A deer whistled loudly, crying “Heu! heu! heu!” as he jumped
away, and on the heels of his cry came a quacking of flushed ducks.

Till that moment I thought I knew where I was; but the quacking brought
doubt, and then bewilderment. If a duck tells you anything in the
woods, he tells you of water, plenty of it; but the map showed no body
of water nearer than Big Pine Pond, which I had fished that day, and
which should be three or four miles behind me. Turning in the direction
of the alarm, I soon broke out of the cover upon a caribou bog, a
mysterious expanse never before suspected in that region, and before me
was the gleam of water in the sunshine. “A pond, a new one, and what a
beauty!” I thought with elation, as I caught its awakening look and
feasted my eyes on its glory of color. Then I gave it an Indian name
and hurried away; for I was surely off my course, and the hour was late
for lingering in strange woods. Somewhere to the west of me was the
home lake; so westward I headed, making a return-compass of bent twigs,
till I set my feet in a branch of the old logging-road. And that chance
trail is the one I have ever since followed.








XI

WOODSY IMPRESSIONS


Next morning I returned to explore my find at leisure. One part of that
exploration was to go completely around the bog, to learn its guiding
landmarks and compass-bearings; but an earlier and better part was to
sit quietly beside my pond to hear whatever it might have to say to me.
If that last sounds fanciful, remember that many things are voiceless
in this world, but few are wholly dumb. Of the numberless ponds that
brighten the northern wilderness, some were made by beavers, others by
flood or glacier or earthquake, and no two of them tell the same story
or make the same impression. They are like so many unspoiled Indians,
whom we regard from a distance as being mysteriously alike, but who
have different traditions, ideals, personalities, and even different
languages.

I know not what the spell of any lonely place may be when you make
yourself part of it; I only know that it stirs one strangely, like the
flute note of a wood-thrush or a song without words. Though I never met
with an adventure on my little pond, never cast a fly to learn whether
any trout lurked in its waters, never thought of firing a shot at its
abundant game, yet season after season I returned to it expectantly,
and went away satisfied. Such a pond has a charm of its own, a spell
which our forebears sought to express in terms of nymphs or puckwudgies
or water-sprites. It grows a better crop than trout, attracts a finer
game than deer or water-fowl, and you can seldom visit it without
learning something new about your natural self or the wood folk or the
friendly universe.

Thus, it happens on a day when you are waiting beside your pond, or
wending your way to it, that a moose or a fox or a dainty grouse
appears unexpectedly near you; and instantly, without thought or
motive, you “freeze” in your tracks or, if you are not seen, shrink
deeper into the shadow for concealment. The action is natural,
involuntary, instinctive, precisely like the action of a young deer
under similar circumstances; but when it is over you understand it, and
smile at finding yourself becoming more and more like other natural
creatures,—going softly, that is, making yourself inconspicuous without
trying or knowing how, and having no thought of harm to any bird or
beast, but only of watching him or gauging his course while remaining
yourself unseen. Only by some such method can you learn anything worth
knowing about a wild animal: books describe, naturalists classify and
sportsmen kill him; but to understand him you must be a sharer of his
quiet ways.

Comes another day, a day when you are in love with solitude itself,
when you learn with surprise that a man is never lonely when alone in
the woods; that ideals may be quite as companionable as folks; and that
around you in a goodly company are beauty, peace, spacious freedom and
harmonious thoughts, with a hint also, to some minds, of angels and
ministers of grace. The Attendant Spirit of “Comus,” the Ariel of “The
Tempest,” the good fairies of all folk,—these are never understood in
the town, nor in the woods unless you enter them alone.

At a later time, and with a thrill of great wonder, you may discover
the meaning of silence, and of the ancient myth of a lovely goddess of
silence; not the dead silence of a dungeon, which may roar in a man’s
ears till it deafens him or drives him mad, but the exquisite living
silence of nature, a silence which at any moment may break into an
elfin ringing of bells, or into a faintly echoing sound of melody, as
if stars or unseen beings were singing far away.

This impression of melody is often real, not illusory, and may be
explained by the impact of air-currents on resonant shells of wood,
hundreds of which fall to humming with the voice of ’cellos and
wind-harps; but there is another experience of the solitude, more
subtle but none the less real, for which only the psychologist will
venture to give an accounting. Once in a season, perhaps, comes an hour
when, no matter what your plans or desires may be, your mind seems
intent on some unrelated affair of its own. As you hurry over the
trail, you may be thinking of catching a trout or stalking a buck or
building a camp or getting to windward of a corporation; meanwhile your
subconscious mind, disdaining your will or your worry, is busily making
pictures of whatever attractive thing it sees,—radiant little pictures,
sunshiny or wind-swept, which shall be reproduced for your pleasure
long after the important matters which then occupied you are clean
forgotten.

Here is the story of one such picture, a reflection, no doubt, of the
primitive trait or quality called place-memory, which enables certain
animals or savages to recognize any spot on which their eyes have once
rested.

One late afternoon, years after I had found my pond, I crossed the
mountain from distant Ragged Lake, heading for the home lake by a new
route. There was no trail; but near the foot of the western slope of
the hills I picked up an old lumber road which seemed to lead in the
right direction. For a time all went well, and confidently; but when
the road dipped into an immense hollow, and there showed signs of
petering out, I followed it with increasing doubt, not knowing where I
might come out of the woods or be forced to spend the night. As I
circled through a swale, having left the road to avoid a press of
alders that filled it, an ash-tree lifted its glossy head above a
thicket with a cheery “Well met again, pilgrim! Whither away now?”

It was a surprising hail in that wild place, suggestive of dreams or
sleep-walking; but under the illusion was a grain of reality which
brought me to an instant halt. After passing under thousands of silent
trees all day, suddenly here was one speaking to me. And not only that,
but wearing a familiar look, like a face which smiles its recognition
of you while you try in vain to place it. Where, when had I seen that
tree before? No, impossible! I had never before entered this part of
the vast forest. Yet I must have seen it somewhere, or it could not now
stir a familiar memory. Nonsense! just a trick of the imagination. I
must hurry on. Thus my thoughts ran, like a circling hare; and all the
while the ash-tree seemed to be smiling at my perplexity.

The man who ignores such a hint has much to learn about woodcraft,
which is largely a subconscious art; so I sat down to smoke a
council-pipe with myself and the ash-tree over the matter. No sooner
was the mind left to its own unhampered way than it began to piece bits
of a puzzle-picture deftly together; and when the picture was complete
I knew exactly where I was, and where I might quickly find a familiar
trail. Eight years before, in an idle hour when nothing stirred on my
pond, I had explored a mile or so beyond the bog to the south, only to
find a swampy, desolate country without a trail or conspicuous landmark
of any kind. It was while I passed through this waste, seeking nothing
in particular and returning to my pond, that the mind took its snapshot
of a certain tree, and preserved the picture so carefully, so minutely,
that years later the original was instantly recognized. Many similar
ash-trees grew on that flat, each with its glossy crown and its gray
shaft flecked by dark-green moss; what there was in this one to attract
me, what outward grace or inward tree-sprite, I have not yet found out.

Another subconscious record seems to have been made for beauty alone,
with its consequent pleasure, rather than for utility. As I watched my
pond one summer morning, intent on learning what attracted so many deer
to its shores, the mind apparently chose its own moment for making a
perfect picture, a masterpiece, which should hang in its woodsy frame
on my mental wall forever. The sky was wondrously clear, the water
dancing, the air laden with the fragrance of peat and sweet-scented
grass. Deer were slow in coming that morning, and meanwhile nothing of
consequence stirred on my pond; but there was still abundant
satisfaction in the brilliant dragon-flies that balanced on bending
reeds, or in the brood of wild ducks that came bobbing out like young
mischief—makers from a hidden bogan, or even in the face of the pond
itself, as it brightened under a gleam of sunshine or frowned at a
passing cloud or broke into a laugh at the touch of a cat’s-paw wind.
Suddenly all these pleasant minor matters were brushed aside when a
bush quivered and held still on the farther shore.

All morning the bushes had been quivering, showing the silvery side of
their leaves to every breeze; but now their motion spoke of life, and
spoke truly, for out from under the smitten bilberries came a bear to
stand alert in the open. The fore part of his body was lifted up as he
planted his paws on a tussock; his massive head was thrust forward as
he tried to penetrate the far distance with his near-sighted eyes. He
was not suspicious, not a bit; his nose held steady as a pointing
dog’s, instead of rocking up and down, as it does when a bear tries to
steal a message from the air. A moment he poised there, a statue of
ebony against the crimson moss; then he leaped a bogan with surprising
agility, and came at his easy, shuffling gait around a bend of the
shore. Opposite me he sat down to cock his nose at the sky, twisting
his head as he followed the motion of something above him, which I
could not see,—a hornet, perhaps, or a troublesome fly that persisted
in buzzing about his ears. Twice he struck quickly with a paw,
apparently missing the lively thing overhead; for he jumped up, rushed
ahead violently and spun around on the pivot of his toes. Then he
settled soberly to his flat-footed shuffle once more, and disappeared
in a clump of larches, which seemed to open a door for him as he drew
near.

For me that little comedy was never repeated, though I saw many another
on dark days or bright; and the last time I visited my pond I beheld it
sadly altered, its beauty vanished, its shores flooded, its green trees
stark and dead. Unknown to me, however, the mind had made its
photographic record, and always I see my pond, as on that perfect day,
in its setting of misty-green larches and crimson bog. Again its quiet
face changes, like a human face at pleasant thoughts, and over it comes
to me the odor of sweet-scented grass. The sunshine brightens it; the
clouds shadow it; brilliant dragon-flies play among its bending reeds;
the same brood of ducklings glides in or out from bogan to grassy
bogan; and forever the bear, big and glossy black, goes shuffling along
the farther shore.








XII

LARCH-TREES AND DEER


One of the subtler charms of my pond, a thing felt rather than seen,
was a certain air of secrecy which seldom left it. In every wilderness
lake lurks a mystery of some kind, which you cannot hope to
penetrate,—a sense of measureless years, of primal far-off things, of
uncouth creatures dead and gone that haunted its banks before the
infancy of man; but on this little pond, with its sunny waters and open
shore, the mystery was always pleasant, and at times provoking, as if
it might be the place where an end of the rainbow rested.

Though small enough to give one a sense of possession (one can never
feel that he owns a big lake, or anything else which gives an
impression of grandeur or sublimity), my pond had a mischievous way of
hinting, when you were most comfortable, that it was hiding a secret;
that it might show you, if it would, a much better scene than that you
looked upon. It was shaped somewhat like an immense pair of spectacles,
having two lobes that were flashing bright, with a narrow band of
darker water between; and, what with its bending shores or intervening
larches, you could never see the whole of it from any one place. So,
like eyes that hide their subtlest lights of whim or fancy under
glasses, it often seemed to be holding something in reserve, something
which it would not reveal unless you searched for it. After watching
awhile from one beautiful or restful spot, you began to feel or imagine
that some comedy was passing unseen on the other half of the pond; and
though you resisted the feeling at first, sooner or later you crept
through the screen of larches to know if it were true.

On every side of the pond save one, where a bank of evergreen made
velvet shadows intermingled with spots of heavenly blue, the shores
were thickly spread with mosses, which began to color gloriously in
midsummer, the colors deepening as the season waned, till the
reflecting water appeared as the glimmering center of a gorgeous
Oriental rug. Along the edges of this rug, as a ragged fringe, stood
groups of larches in irregular order,—little fairylike larches that
bore their crown of leaves not as other trees bear them, heavily, but
as a floating mist or nebula of sage green. Like New England ladies of
a past age they seemed, each wearing a precious lace shawl which gave
an air of daintiness to their sterling worth. When the time came for
the leaves to fall, instead of rustling down to earth with a sound of
winter, mournfully, they would scamper away on a merry wind, mingling
their fragrance with that of the ripened grass; and then the twigs
appeared plainly for the first time, with a little knot or twist in
every twig, like toil-worn fingers that the lace had concealed.

Here or there amid this delicate new growth towered the ruin of a
mighty tamarack, or ship-knee larch, such as men sought in the old
clipper-ship days when they needed timbers lighter than oak, and even
tougher to resist the pressure of the gale or the waves’ buffeting.
Once, before the shipmen penetrated thus far into the wilderness, the
tamaracks stood here in noble array, their heads under clouds,
beckoning hungry caribou to feed from the lichens that streamed from
their broad arms above the drifted snow; now most of them are under the
moss, which covered them tenderly when they fell. The few remaining
ones stand as watch-towers for the hawks and eagles; their broken
branches make strange sepia drawings of dragon-knots and hooked beaks
on the blue sky. A tiny moth killed all these great larches; the
caribou moved northward, leaving the country, and the deer moved in to
take possession.

This and many other stories of the past my little pond told me, as I
watched from its shores or followed the game-trails that were spread
like a net about its edges. Back in the woods these trails wandered
about in devious fashion, seeking good browse or easy traveling; while
here or there a faint outgoing branch offered to lead you, if your eyes
were keen, to the distant ridge where a big buck had his daily
loafing-place. On the bog the trails went more circumspectly, uniting
at certain places in a single deep path, a veritable path of ages,
which was the only path that might safely be followed by any creature
with more weight than a fox. The moment you ventured away from it the
ground began to shiver, to quake alarmingly, to sink down beneath your
feet. Only a thin mat of roots kept you afloat; the roots might
anywhere part and drop you into black bottomless ooze, and close
forever over your head. A queer place, one might think, for heavy
beasts to gather, and so it was; but the old caribou-trails or new
deer-paths offered every one of them safe footing.

At first these game-trails puzzled me completely, being so many and so
pointless. That they were in constant use was evident from the
footprints in them, which were renewed almost every morning; yet I
never once saw a deer approach the water to drink or feed. Something
else attracted them; a highway from one feeding-ground to another, it
might be, or the wider outlook which brings deer and caribou out of
their dim woods to sightly places; but there was no certainty in the
matter until the animals themselves revealed the secret. One day, when
a young buck passed my hiding-place as if he were going somewhere, I
followed him to the upper or southern end of the pond. There he joined
four other deer, which were very busy about a certain spot, half hidden
by low bushes, a couple of hundred yards back from the shore. And there
they stayed, apparently eating or drinking, for a full half-hour or
more.

When the deer were gone away, I went over and found a huge spring, to
which converged a dozen deep trails. Like the hub of an immense wheel
it seemed: the radiating paths were the spokes, and somewhere beyond
the horizon was the unseen rim. From the depths of the spring came a
surprising volume of clear, coffee-colored water, bubbling over
joyously as it leaped from the dark earth into the light, and then
stealing quietly away under bending grasses to keep my pond brim full.
Around the spring the earth was pitted by the feet of deer, and
everywhere about its edges were holes lapped in the peat by eager
tongues. Here, beyond a doubt, was what called so many animals to my
pond,—a mineral spring or salt-lick, such as we read about in stories
of pioneer days, when game was everywhere abundant, but such as one now
rarely finds.

After that happy discovery I shifted my blind to another larch with
low-drooping branches, beneath which one might rest comfortably and
look out through a screen of lace upon a gathering of the deer. They
are creatures of habit as well as of freedom; and one of their habits
is to rest at regular intervals, the hours being hard to forecast,
since they vary not only with the season of lengthening or shortening
days, but also each month with the changes of the moon. Thus, when the
moon fulls and weather is clear, deer are abroad most of the night. At
dawn they seek their day-beds, instinctively removing far from where
they have left their scent in feeding; and during the day they are apt
to remain hidden save for one brief hour, when they take a comforting
bite here or there, giving the impression that they eat now from habit
rather than from hunger. As the moon wanes they change their hours to
take advantage of its shining; and on the “dark of the moon” they
browse only in the early part of the night, then rest many hours, and
have two periods of feeding or roaming the next day.

Such seems to be the rule in the North, with plenty of exceptions to
keep one guessing,—as in the November mating-season, when bucks are
afoot at all hours; or during a severe storm, which keeps deer and all
other wild animals close in their coverts.

Because of this regularity of habit at irregular hours, the only
certainty about the salt-lick was that the animals would come if one
waited long enough. As I watched expectantly from my larch bower, the
morning shadows might creep up to me, halt, and lengthen away on the
other side, while not a deer showed himself in the open. Then there
would be a stir in the distant larches, a flash of bright color; a doe
would emerge from one of the game-trails, hastening her springy steps
as she neared the spring. As my eyes followed her, noting with pleasure
her graceful poses, her unwearied alertness, her frequent turning of
the head to one distant spot in the woods where she had left her fawn,
there would come another flash of color from another trail, then two or
three in a flecking of light and shadow, till half a dozen or more deer
were gathered at the lick, some lapping the mud eagerly, others
sipping, sipping, as if they could never have enough of the water.
After a time they would slip away as they had come, singly or in
groups; the spring would be deserted, and one could never tell how many
hours or days might pass before another company began to gather.

However eager for salt they might be, the deer came or went in that
mysteriously silent way of theirs, appearing without warning in one
trail, or vanishing down another without a sound to mark their passing.
Now and then, however, especially if one watched at the exquisite
twilight hour, a very different entrance might be staged on the lonely
bog,—a gay, prancing “here I come: get out of the way” kind of
entrance, which made one glad he had stayed to witness it. On the slope
of the nearest ridge your eye would catch an abrupt motion, the upward
surge of a bough or the spring-back of a smitten bush; presently to
your ears would come a rapid thudding of earth, or a sqush, sqush,
sqush of water; the larches would burst open and a buck leap forth,
flourishing broad antlers or kicking up mad heels as he went gamboling
down the game-trail. If other deer were at the spring, they would throw
up their heads, set their ears at the dancing buck, take a last quick
sip from the spring, and move aside as he jumped in to muzzle the mud
as if famished. For it was the mud rather than the water which first
claimed his attention, no doubt because it held more of the magic salt.
He often gave the impression, as he approached in high feather, that he
had been tasting the stuff in anticipation and could hardly wait to get
his tongue into it.

The first time I saw that frisky performance I went over to taste the
mud for myself, but found little to distinguish it from the mud of any
other peat-bog. The water from the spring was wholesome, with a faint
taste of something I could not name; and I drank it repeatedly without
learning its secret. That it held a charm of some kind, which chemistry
might reveal, was evident from the fact that deer came from miles
around to enjoy its flavor. Some of the trails could be traced clear
across the bog to distant ridges and a broken country beyond; and in
following these trails, to learn what creatures used them and where
they came from, I repeatedly came upon a deer asleep in his day-bed.
Whether the animals couched here before drinking at the spring, or
after drinking, or “just by happentry” I could not tell.

Once the sleeper was a buck with noble antlers. He was resting beside a
great log on the edge of an opening, half surrounded by dense fir
thickets. I speak of him as asleep; but that is mere habit of speech or
poverty of language. Of a score of wild birds or beasts that I have
found “asleep” in the woods, not one seemed to lose touch with the
waking world even for an instant. The buck’s eyelids were blinking, his
head nodding heavily; yet all the while his feet were curled in
readiness for an instant jump; and somehow those expressive feet gave
the impression of being as wide awake as a squirrel. Occasionally as I
watched him, fascinated by the rare sight, his head would drop almost
to the ground, only to be jerked up with an air of immense surprise;
then the sleepy fellow would stare in a filmy, unseeing, “who said I
was asleep” kind of way at a little tree that stood in the opening. The
stare would end with a slow closing of the eyelids, and in a moment he
would be nodding again.








XIII

BLACK MALLARDS


Next to the deer, the wild ducks were the chief attraction of my pond.
Indeed, they might well be placed first, since they were always at home
there, and much of the time engaged in one or another of the little
comedies that make ducks the most amusing of all birds. Eight summers
in succession, and again after an interval of two years, I found my
pond occupied by a pair of black mallards with their brood; and I
fancied, since migratory birds return to the place of their birth, and
their nestlings after them, that one of the pair was the lineal
descendant of ducks that had held the place in undisputed possession
for tens of thousands of years. Here was a succession, modest like all
true nobility, which made the proud family trees of Mayflower folk or
English kings or Norman barons look like young berry-bushes in the
shade of a towering pine.

Until late midsummer the family had the pond all to themselves. Never a
stranger-duck appeared to share or challenge their heritage; while day
after day the mother watched over the little brood as they fed or
played or learned the wild-duck signals. Like our dogs, every manner of
beast or bird has its own tribal ways or customs, some of which do not
appear in the young until they begin to roam abroad or to mingle with
their kind. So, as I watched the brood emerge from down to
pin-feathers, there would come a red-letter day when two of them,
meeting as they rounded a grassy point, would raise their wings as if
in salutation; and a later day when, the pin-feathers having grown to
fair plumage, their young cheepings or whistlings would change to a
decided quack.

Thereafter their talk was endlessly entertaining, if one took the
trouble to creep near enough to appreciate its modulations, expressive
of every emotion between drowsiness and tense alarm; for it cannot be
heard, except as a meaningless sound, beyond a few yards. The little
hen-ducks got on famously, having the mother’s quacking as a model; but
male ducks cannot or will not learn to quack, and since a male voice
was rarely heard on the pond at this season, each little drake was a
law unto himself, and made a brave show of his liberty. Climbing on a
tussock, as if for more room, he would stretch his wings, make odd
motions with his neck, and finally pump out a funny wheekle, wheekle,
as if he had swallowed a whistle.

Meanwhile the old drake and father of the family was seldom about; only
two or three times did I see him enter the pond, stay a brief while,
and then wing away over the tree-tops in the direction of a larger
lake, some three miles to the eastward. On that lake there was never a
brood of young ducks, so far as I could learn; but when trout-fishing
there I often surprised the drake, at times taking precious care of his
own skin in solitude, again clubbing sociably with three or four other
drakes, who had run away each from a family and the cares thereof on
some other lonely pond.

As the summer waned, a new sound of quacking, joyous and exultant,
would greet me when I drew near my pond. Creeping to my blind under the
larches, I would find a second brood making merry acquaintance with the
family I had watched over; then a third and a fourth company of
strangers, as young ducks of all that region began to traffic about in
preparation for the autumn flight. A little later the flocks fairly
reveled in sociability, gathering here or there with increasing
numbers, till on a late-September day I might find my pond deserted,
the owners being on a visit elsewhere, or I might catch breath at sight
of so many ducks that I could not accurately count them or distinguish
one brood from another.

At such a time my little pond seemed to awaken, to shed its silence
like a garment, to put on its most animated expression, as at a happy
festival or family reunion. The air was never still from the gabble of
meeting groups (probably all more or less related), or from the
resounding quank, quank, quank of some old gossip who went about
proclaiming her opinion to the whole company. Everywhere the still
water was broken into undulating wakes as the drakes swept grandly over
it, with that rhythmic, forward-and-back motion of their heads which is
like duck poetry,—a motion that is not seen when the birds are feeding,
but only when they are well satisfied with themselves or their
audience. Through the shadows under the bank glided knots or ribbons of
young birds which had not yet quite satisfied their appetites, some
exploring every crevice for ripened seeds, others tip-tilting their
tails to the blue sky as they probed the bottom for water-bugs and
other titbits. In an open space a solitary hen-duck bobbed and teetered
ecstatically, dipping the fore part of her body under, then heaving it
up quickly so as to send the cleansing water in a foamy wave over her
back and wings. Here or there on a tussock stood a quiet group of the
splendid birds, oiling their glossy feathers, setting a wing-cover just
right, or adding some other last touch to an elaborate toilet before
settling down for a nap.

The glassy water reflected every form, color, motion of these
untroubled ducks as in a glass, doubling the graceful effect. Around
them stretched the gloriously colored bog; and beyond the bog were the
nebulous-green larches, the somber black growth and the lifting hills,
on which autumn had laid its golden touch. Truly a beautiful sight, a
sight to make the heart of hunter or naturalist tremble with expectancy
as he fingered his gun. I have known that trembling, that expectancy;
but there was greater pleasure, perhaps greater freedom also, in
leaving the happy comedy undisturbed.

Because of its solitude, its utter wildness, my pond seemed to be the
chosen resting-place of the flocks on an autumn day (they feed or
travel mostly by night), and perhaps for the same reason the ducks that
frequented it were among the wildest creatures I have ever tried to
stalk. A black mallard is not an easy bird to outwit at any time or
place; but here some magic mirror or sounding-board seemed to
supplement his natural eyes or ears. The slightest unnatural voice or
appearance, the snap of a twig or the quiver of a leaf or the glimpse
of a face in the larches, would send a flock away on the instant; and
sometimes, when I was sure no sound or motion of mine had broken the
perfect quiet, they would take wing in such incomprehensible fashion as
to leave me wondering what extra sense had warned them of danger.

Several times in the course of a summer, when I wanted to observe the
little duck family more nearly, or to learn the meaning of some queer
play that I could not understand from a distance, I would creep out of
the larches unseen, worming my way along a sunken deer-path, and
stopping whenever heads were turned in my direction. One might think it
an easy matter to approach any game by such methods; yet almost
invariably, before I could be safe behind a bush or a tuft of grass at
the water’s edge, the old mother-duck would become uneasy, like a deer
that catches a vague hint of you floating far down the wind. That she
could not see or hear me was certain; that she could not smell me I had
repeatedly proved; nevertheless, after searching the shores narrowly
she would stretch her neck straight up from the water, as if attentive
to some wireless message in the air.

A wild duck does not take that alert attitude unless she is suspicious;
and a curious thing was, that though the mother was silent, uttering
never a word, the young would crouch and remain motionless wherever
they happened to be. Suddenly, as if certain of danger but unable to
locate it, the mother would spring aloft to go sweeping in wide circles
over the bog. She seemed to know it by heart, every pool and bump and
shadow of it; and when her keen eyes picked up an unfamiliar shadow on
a certain deer-path she would come at it with a rush, whirling over it
in an upward-climbing spiral till she became sure of me, as of
something out of place, when she would speed away with a warning note
over the tree-tops. If the young were strong of wing, they would follow
her swiftly, giving wide berth to the deer-path as if she had told them
beware of it; but if they did not yet trust themselves in the air, they
would skulk away, their heads down close to the water, and hide in one
of the grassy bogans of the pond, where because of the quaking shore it
was impossible to come near them.

Once, when the mother left in this way, I waited till the ducklings had
been some minutes hidden before creeping back to my blind in the
larches. An hour or more passed in the timeless quiet; while the water
became as glass under the afternoon sun, and a deer moved near the
hidden brood without flushing them or even bringing a head up where I
could see it. Then the mother returned, calling as she came; and the
first thing she did was to circle warily over the same deer-path,
stretching her neck down for a close inspection. “Aha! that thing is
gone, but where?” she said in every line and motion of her inquisitive
head or pulsating wings, as she sped away to find the answer.

Twice she circled the bog, her eyes searching every cranny and shadow
of it. From her high flight she slanted straight down and pitched fair
in the middle of the pond, where for some moments she sat motionless,
her head up, looking, listening,—a perfect image of alertness in the
midst of wildness. Satisfied at last that no trouble was near, she
turned to the shore with a low call; and out of the bogan pell-mell
rushed the little ones, splashing, cheeping, half lifting themselves
with their tiny wings as they scurried over the water to join the
mother. For a full hour I had kept my glasses almost continuously on
that bogan; then with divided attention I cast expectant glances at it
when I heard the mother’s incoming note, the whish of her wings as she
circled the bog and the splash as she took the water; but not till the
right signal came did I see a motion or a sign of life from the hidden
brood.

The pond was shaped, as we have noticed, like a pair of spectacles; and
a favorite place for the autumn flock to rest or preen or sleep was at
the bend between the two lobes. Down into that bend ran a screen of
alder-bushes, the only good cover between woods and water on the entire
pond; and it was so dense that a cat could hardly have crept through it
without making a disturbance. That was one reason, I suppose, why the
ducks felt safe at the outer end of the tangle: they could see
everything in front or on either side, and hear anything that moved
behind them.

One day, when the shore at this bend was freshly starred by ducks’ feet
and littered with feathers, showing that a large flock had just left
the roost, I began at the fringe of larches and cut a passageway, a
regular beaver’s tunnel, down the whole length of the alder run, making
an end in a point of grass, where the water came close on three sides.
One had to consider only the birds’ keen ears, the alder screen being
so thick that not even a duck’s eye could penetrate it; therefore I
smoothed the way most carefully, leaving no stick below to crack under
my weight, and no branch reaching down to rustle or quiver as I crawled
beneath it. When the tunnel was well finished I left the pond to its
solitude a few days, thinking that the birds would surely notice some
telltale sign of my work, some fresh-cut stick or wilted bough that my
eyes had overlooked, and be wary of the alders for a little time.

And why such pains to get near a bird, you ask, since one might better
observe or shoot him from a comfortable distance? Oh, just a notion of
mine, an odd notion, which can hardly be appreciated till one has
proved it in the open. As you can seldom “feel” the quality of a
stranger while he remains even a few yards away, so with any wild bird
or beast: there is an impression arising from nearness, from contact,
which cannot be had in any other way; and that swift impression, which
is both physical and mental, a judgment as it were of the entire
nature, is often more illuminating than hours of ordinary observation
or speculation.

Such an impression is not new or strange, or even modernly
psychological. On the contrary, it is the simplest matter in the world
of sense, I think, and perhaps also the surest. Most animals have a
significant way of touching their noses to one of their own kind at
meeting; not to smell him, as we imagine (they can smell him, or even
his tracks, at a distance), but in order to receive a more intimate or
convincing message than the sense of smell can furnish. Likewise, a man
naturally pats the head of a dog, or fingers an object after minutely
scanning it with his eyes; and in this instinctive action is the
ancient touch of recognition. Touch is the oldest and most universal of
the bodily senses, sight, smell, taste and hearing being later
specializations thereof; by it the living creature first became aware
of a world outside of self; and to it we all return for verification of
our sense impressions. Therefore it happens most naturally that,
despite warning signs or penalties, thoughtless men will put their
hands into the bear or monkey cage, where animals are no longer natural
or to be trusted, and our children must be forever lectured, or
sometimes spanked, for handling things which they have been told to let
alone.

Besides, when one is very near a strange bird or beast, one becomes
vaguely conscious of an extra sense at work,—that real but uncatalogued
sense-of-presence (to coin a name for it) which makes two persons in a
room aware of each other at every instant, even while both are absorbed
in quiet work or reading. The “feel” of the same room when one occupies
it alone is very different; and the difference may help to explain why
gregarious animals are uncomfortable, uneasy, unless they are near
their own kind,—near enough, that is, not simply to hear or see them
but to feel their bodily presence. A herd-animal is always restless,
and often sickens, if his herd is not close about him. The same
mysterious sense (mysterious to us, because we do not yet know the
organ through which it works) often warns the solitary man in the woods
or in the darkness that some living creature is near him, at a moment
when his eyes or ears are powerless to verify his impression.

But that is another and more subtle matter, familiar enough to a few
sensitive persons and natural woodsmen, but impossible of demonstration
to others; you cannot explain color to a man born blind. The simple
answer is, that for my own satisfaction I wanted to touch one of the
wary birds of my pond, as I had before touched eagle and crow, bear and
deer, and many another wild creature in his native woods. Such was the
notion. In other places I had several times tried to indulge it; but
save in one instance, when I found a winter flock weakened by hunger, I
had never laid my hand fairly on a black mallard when he had the free
use of his wits and wings.

When I returned to my pond, and from a distance swept my glasses over
it, the water was alive with ducks; never before had I seen so many
there at one time. Single large birds, the drakes undoubtedly, were
moving leisurely over the open spaces. Groups of five or six, each a
brood from some neighboring pond, were gliding in an exploring kind of
way under the banks or through the weed-beds; and scattered along the
shore at the end of the alder run were wisps or companies of the birds,
all preening or dozing with an air of complete security. Here at last
was my chance, my perfect chance, I told myself, as I carefully marked
one brood standing at the tip of the grassy point where my tunnel
ended.

More carefully than ever I stalked a bear, I circled through the black
growth, crept under the fringe of larches, and entered the alder run
unobserved. Inch by inch I wormed along the secret passageway, flat to
the ground, not once raising my head, hardly daring to pull a full
breath, till, just as I emerged from the alder shade into the grass, a
gamy scent in my nose and a low gabble in my ears told me that I was
almost near enough, that the birds were all around me, and that for the
rest of the way I must move as a shadow.

From under my hat-brim I located the gabblers, a large family of black
mallards outside the fringe of grass on my left. They were abreast of
me, not more than five or six feet away. I had not marked these birds
when I began my stalk; they were hidden in a tiny cove or bend of the
shore, and had it not been for their voices I would surely have crept
past without seeing them. At the mouth of the cove was a single
tussock, on which stood the mother-duck, wavering between dreams and
watchfulness as the sunshine poured full upon her, making her very
sleepy. On the bare earth beneath her the others were getting ready for
a nap, so near that I could see every motion, the settling of a head,
the blink of an eyelid. Occasionally through the tangle of grass stems
came the penetrating gleam of their eyes,—marvelously bright eyes,
alert and intelligent.

For several minutes I held motionless, still flat to the ground,
listening to the sleepy talk, admiring the mottled-brown plumage of a
breast or the bar of brilliant color drawn athwart a sooty wing. All
the while my nose was trying to get in a warning word, telling me to
give heed that the ducky odor which flowed in waves over the whole
point was different from this strong reek, as of a disturbed nest, in
the near-by grass; but my eyes were so occupied that I paid no
attention to other senses. As the duck on the tussock at last settled
down to sleep and I worked my toes into the earth for a noiseless push
forward, there was a slight but startling motion almost at my shoulder.
A neck was raised and twisted sleepily, as if to get the kink out of
it; and the thrill of success ran over me as I made out another and
nearer group of ducks. They were under the bank and the bending grass,
where I had completely overlooked them. Every one was within reach; and
every one I could see had his head drawn in or tucked away under his
wing.

Slowly my left hand stole toward them, creeping forward in the
deliberate fashion of a measuring-worm, first the fingers stretched,
then the knuckles raised, then out with the fingers again. It would
have been very easy to stroke or to catch one of the birds by a swift
motion; but that was not what I wanted, and would have instantly
spoiled the whole comedy. For the right effect, the hand must rest upon
a duck before he was aware of it, so quietly that at first he would
give his attention to the hand itself, not to the thing it came from.
Then he would probably give it a questioning peck, examine it
curiously, and finally grow indifferent to it, as other birds had done
when I touched them from hiding. But here my head was too close to the
ground, and my body too cramped for easy action. As my hand reached the
edge of the bank, just over an unconscious duck, it ran into a tuft of
saw-grass, which cut my fingers and rustled dangerously. To clear this
obstruction I drew back slightly, lifted up a grain; and in my other
ear, which was turned away, a resonant voice cried Quock! with a
challenge that broke the tension like a pistol-shot.

Involuntarily I turned my head, just when I should have held most
still; and so I lost my chance. There, at arm’s-length on the other
side of the point, a wild-eyed duck was looking over the bank, her neck
stretched like a taut string, her olive-colored bill pointing straight
at me. She never said another word, and had no need to repeat her
challenge. All over the point and along the shore necks were stretched
up from the grass; a dozen alert forms rose like sentinels from as many
tussocks, and forty pairs of keen eyes were every one searching the
spot at which the old hen-duck pointed her accusation.

For a small moment that tableau lasted, without a sound, without a
motion; while one was conscious only of the tense necks, the pointing
bills, the gleaming little eyes, each with its diamond-point of light;
and then the old duck took wing. She did not even crouch to jump, so
far as I could follow her motion; she simply went into the air like a
rocket, shooting aloft as if hurled from a spring. As she rose, there
was an answering rush of wings, whoosh! in my very ears, a surge as of
smitten water in the distance; and in the same fraction of an instant
every duck to the farthest ends of the pond was up and away in a wild
tumult of quacking.

Only one of these birds had seen me, and that one probably had no
notion of what she had glimpsed in the grass. It was a round thing with
eyes, and it moved a second time—that was enough for the old hen-duck,
and the others did not stop to ask any questions.








XIV

MEMORIES


Two full years passed before I returned to my pond on a sunny September
day, in my mind’s eye seeing it smile a welcome, hearing it cry, “Lo
here! Lo there!” and planning, as I came down the silent trail, how I
would accept all its invitations. First, the salt-lick must be spied
out from a distance; and the examination would tell me whether to keep
on down my own trail or, if the lick were occupied, to branch off by a
certain game-path, which would lead me to the blind where I had so
often watched the deer unseen. Next, I would have a restful look at a
mound of moss swelling above the bog near a certain tamarack, which
always showed the first blush of crimson in midsummer, and which became
in autumn like a gorgeous bed of Dutch tulips, only more wondrously
colored. Then I would look into the doorway under the larches, where my
bear had disappeared. I always picked that out from a hundred similar
doorways to watch or question it a moment, as if at any time the green
curtain might open to let the bear out. For a curious thing about all
woodsmen is this: if they see a buck or a bear or even a fox enter a
certain place, they must forever afterward stop to have another
expectant look at it.

From the bear’s doorway my thoughts turned naturally to a little bogan
of my pond, which was different from all the other bogans, because once
a family of minks darted out of it and came dodging along the shore in
my direction. Luckily I was close to the water at that moment. While
the minks were out of sight under some bushes, I swung my feet over the
bank and sat down in their path to wait for them.

In advance came the mother, looking rusty in her sunburnt summer coat,
and she was evidently in a great hurry about something. The little
ones, trailing out behind, were hard put to it to keep up the pace. She
was fairly under me before she noticed a new scent in the air, which
made her halt to look about for the meaning of it. Her neck was lifted,
weasel-fashion, to thrice its ordinary length; at the end of it her
pointed head swung like a vane to the bank, to the pond, to the bank
again; while her busy nose wiggled out its sharp questions. Probably
she had no notion of man, never having met the creature; neither did
she associate the motionless figure above her with life or danger. She
passed directly over one of my shoes, halted with her paws raised
against the other, and scampered on as if she had no use for such
trifles.

Before the little ones arrived I half turned to meet them, spreading my
feet so as to leave a narrow passageway between the heels; and over
this, as a cover, rested my hand, making a shadowy runway such as minks
like. When the kits entered it, sleek and glossy and half grown, I
touched them lightly on the neck, feeling the soft brush of fur and the
ripple of elastic muscles as one after another glided under my finger,
with no more concern than if it had been one of the roots among which
they were accustomed to creep. But when the last one came I blocked the
runway by placing a hand squarely across it, stopping him short in
great astonishment. He sniffed at the obstruction, and his nose was
like a point of ice as it wandered over my palm. Then he tried a finger
with his teeth, wriggled under it to follow his leader, and the whole
family disappeared in a twisting, snakelike procession around the next
bend. These were wild animals, remember; and ounce for ounce there is
no more “savage” beast in the woods than Cheokhes the mink.

As with birds or beasts, so also with the trees about my pond: somehow
they seemed different from all other trees, perhaps because of more
intimate association; for though all the cedars or hemlocks of a forest
look alike to a stranger, no sooner do you spend days alone among them
than you begin to have a curious feeling of individuality, of
comradeship, of understanding even, as if they were not wholly dumb or
insensate. It was inevitable, therefore, as I came down the trail,
recalling this or that tree under which I had often passed or rested,
that certain of them stood forth in memory as having given me pleasure
or greeting in the lonely woods, just as certain faces emerge from the
sea of faces in a crowd or a great audience of strangers, and instantly
make one feel his kinship to humanity.

Foremost among these memorable trees was a great white-pine, to me the
noblest of all forest growths, which stood on a knoll to westward of my
pond, on the way to camp, and which always seemed to cry hail or
farewell as I came or went. It had a stem to make one wonder, almost to
make one reverent. Massive, soft-colored, finely reticulated it was;
wide as the span of a man’s arms, and rising near a hundred feet
without knot or branch,—a glorious upspringing shaft, immensely strong,
yet delicate in its poise as a lance in rest. From the top of the shaft
rugged arms were stretched out above the tallest trees, and on these
rested lightly as a cloud its crown of green. Like others that overtop
their fellows, the old pine had paid the penalty of greatness.
Whirlwinds that left lower trees untouched had stripped it of half its
branches; lightning had leaped upon it from the clouds, leaving a
spiral scar from crown to foot; but the wound which threatened its
death was meanwhile its life, because the lumbermen, seeing the
lightning’s mark, had passed on and left the pine in its solitary
grandeur.

When I first saw that tree I changed the trail so as to pass beneath
it; and thereafter it was like a living presence, benign and friendly,
beside the way. To lay a hand on its mighty stem, as one passed
eastward in the early morning, was to receive an impression of renewed
power,—a power which the scornful might attribute to imagination, the
chemist to electrons or radio-activity, and the simple man to his
Mother Nature. At evening, as one followed the dim trail homeward in
the fading light, one had only to look up for a guiding sign; and
there, solemn and still against the twilight splendor, was the crown of
the old pine to give direction. Its very silence at such an hour was
like the Angelus ringing. To halt beneath it, as one often did
unconsciously, was to feel the spell of its age, its serenity, its
peace; while harmonious thoughts came or went attuned to the low melody
of the winds, crooning their vesper song far up among its green leaves.
And, morning or midday or evening, to look up at the pine’s lofty
crown, which had tossed in the free winds that bore Pilgrim and Puritan
westward with their immortal dream of freedom, was to be bound with
stronger ties of loyalty to the fathers of my native state,—men of
vision and imagination as well as of stern courage, who heard the pine
booming out its psalm to the gale and instantly adopted it as their new
symbol, stamping it on their coins or emblazoning it on their banners
as an emblem of liberty. Never another symbol, whether dragon or eagle
or lion, had so much majesty, or was so worthy of free men. The
remembrance of it in any national crisis or call to duty sets the
American heart beating to the rhythm of Whittier’s “Pine-Tree”:


    Lift again the stately emblem on the Bay State’s rusted shield,
    Give to northern winds the Pine-Tree on our banner’s tattered
        field.
    Sons of men who sat in council with their Bibles round the board,
    Answering England’s royal missive with a firm “Thus saith the
        Lord,”
    Rise again for home and freedom! set the battle in array!
    What the fathers did of old time we their sons must do to-day.


Very different from the majestic pine was a little larch-tree, under
which I often sat while watching the deer. As I came down the trail,
after a year’s absence, it would seem to lift its head and step forth
from all the other larches, calling out cheerily: “Welcome once more!
And why so long away? See, here is your old place waiting.” And drawing
aside the delicate branches, I would find the seat of dry moss and
springy boughs, the back-rest, the open window with its drapery of
lace,—everything just as I had left it.

Near this sociable young larch stood its dead ancestor, grim and
silent, which the moths had killed; and this, too, seemed different
from all other trees living or dead. On sunny days it threw a straight
shaft of shadow over my blind; and the shadow moved along the ground
from west to east, telling the creeping hours like a sun-dial. At the
tip of the lofty stub a short branch thrust itself out at a right
angle, and this served as the finger of my strange timepiece. When it
rested on a bed of brimming pitcher-plants it pointed to the lunch in
my pocket; when it touched the root of a water-maple it spoke of the
home trail; and between, at irregular intervals, were a nanny-bush, a
tuft of wild cotton and a shy orchid to mark the less important hours.
Once, when I glanced at the slow-moving shadow, it was topped by a
striking symbolic figure, and looking up quickly I found an eagle
perched on the outstretched finger of my dial. After that the old
tamarack had a new dignity in my eyes; it stood on an eagle’s line of
flight, one of his regular ways in crossing from mountain to lake, and
from it the kingly-looking bird was wont to survey this part of his
silent domain, the sun gleaming on his snow-white crest.

A stone’s-throw behind my larch blind was a portly young fir, which I
could never pass without a smile as it nodded to remind me that it was
not like other firs. Thousands of these trees, crowding the northern
forest, seem to be all grown on the same model, like peas in a pod; but
this one had a character and a history to set it forever apart from its
kind. And this is the tale which always passed silently between us when
we met:

One day, as I watched some deer at the salt-lick, they suddenly became
uneasy, looking and harking about as if for danger, and then vanished
down the several game-trails. Not till they were gone did I notice that
the air was ominously still, or understand the cause of the alarm: a
tempest was coming, and the sensitive animals were away to cover before
my dull senses had picked up the first warning sign. Soon the landscape
darkened; the face of my pond became as I had never seen it before;
thunder growled in the distance; coppery clouds with light flaming
through them came rolling over the tree-tops; and all nature said, as
plainly as a fire-bell, “Get to cover, and quickly!”

As I went back into the woods, seeking shelter, a few big drops hit the
leaves like flails; then came a pause, still as death, and then the
deluge. Ahead in the gloom I spied a young fir (never pick a tall tree,
or a solitary tree, in a tempest of lightning) which thrust out a mass
of feathery branches from a thicket of its fellows. “This for mine,” I
said as I dived under it, accompanied by a blinding flare of light and
an ear-splitting crack—and almost ran against the heels of a buck that
jumped out on the other side. By an odd chance, one in ten thousand, he
had picked the same fir for shelter, and was no doubt thinking he had
picked well when I came blundering in with the thunderbolt and drove
him out into the downpour. “Hold on, old sport! Come back; it’s your
tree,” I called after him, feeling as if I had stolen a child’s
umbrella; but he paid no attention.

Thinking he would not go far, and knowing he could hear or smell
nothing in that rush of rain and crashing of thunder, I crept slowly
after him. There he was, hunched up in the lee of a big hemlock, ears
drooping, legs streaming, and little spurts of mist popping up from his
pelted hide. Though woebegone enough, he had not forgotten caution; oh
no! trust an old buck for that in any weather. His tail was to the
tree, his head turned warily to the trail over which he had come. And
there I left him, wishing as I turned back that he would let me stand
under his hemlock, or else come and share my fir, just for a little
company.

Near the lower end of my pond was still another tree which I must
revisit; yes, surely, not only for its happy memories, but also in
anticipation of some merry surprise, of which it seemed to have endless
store. It stood on a bank overlooking a sunny dell in the woods, a
wonderfully pleasant place where no wind entered, where the air was
always fragrant, and a runlet of cool water sang a little tune to
itself all day long. Its gnarled trunk was scarcely more than a shell,
which boomed like a drum when a woodpecker sounded it; and above were
hollow limbs with knot-hole entrances, offering hospitality to any wild
creature in search of a weather-proof den or nesting-place.

The first time I passed this old tree a family of red squirrels were
laying claim to it in a tiff with some larger beast or bird, which
slipped away as I approached. The next time I saw it, a year later, it
was silent and apparently deserted; but as I rose from drinking at the
runlet the head of a little gray owl appeared at a knot-hole. For full
ten minutes he remained there motionless, without word or sign or even
a blink to say that he was watching me, though it was undoubtedly some
noise or stir of mine which brought him up to his window.

After that I fell in the way of turning aside to loaf awhile under the
inn-tree; and rarely could one loaf there very long without overhearing
something not intended for a stranger’s ear, some low dialogue or
hammering signal or petulant whining or cautious scratching, to remind
one of the running comedy of the woods. It was evidently an exchange, a
crossroad or meeting-place for the wood folk, calling in every
passer-by as a certain store or corner of a sleepy town invites all
idlers, boys and stray dogs, while other stores or corners are empty,
save for women folk, and quite respectable.

Once in the late morning, as I sat with an ear to the resonant shell,
listening to the talk of unseen creatures which I fancied were young
’coons, a big log-cock flashed into the old tree, drew himself up on a
stub over my head, and seemed to cock his ear at the voices to which I
had been listening.

Now the log-cock is naturally a wary bird, shy and difficult of
approach; but this gorgeous fellow with the scarlet crest became almost
sociable in his curiosity, perhaps because the place was so quiet, so
friendly, with no motion or hint of danger to disturb its tranquillity.
He saw me at once, as the change in his bright eye plainly said; but,
deceived by my stillness or the sober coloring of my clothes, he set me
down as a tree-fungus or mushroom that had grown since his last visit,
and looked about for something more interesting. When I called his
attention by a curt nod, telling him that this was no dull mushroom, he
came down at once to light against the side of the tree, where he
examined my head minutely. Learning nothing from my wink, he went
around the tree in a series of side-jumps to have a look from the other
side; then he hopped up and down, this side or that, all the while
uttering a low surprised chatter. Even when I began to flip bits of
wood at him (for he soon grew impatient, and interrupted the ’coon talk
by an unseemly rapping), instead of rushing off in alarm, he twice
followed a missile that rattled near him, as if to demand, “Well, what
in the world sent you flying?” Presently he sent forth a call, not the
loud, high, prolonged note which you hear from him at a distance, but a
soft, wheedling ah-koo! ah-koo! only twice repeated. When his call was
answered in a different strain, a questioning strain it seemed to me,
he darted away and returned within the minute accompanied by another
log-cock.

But enough of such pictures! They flash joyously upon the mental vision
whenever one recalls a cherished spot in the woods, but fade quickly if
one attempts to hold or describe them, saying as they vanish that the
lure of solitary lakes, the companionship of trees, the fascination of
wild creatures that hide and look forth with roundly curious eyes at a
stranger’s approach,—these are matters that can never be set down in
words: the best always escapes in the telling. I meant only to say
(when my pine lifted its crown in the light of an evening sky, and then
the mink family came dodging along the shore of memory, and the buck
and the log-cock interrupted to urge me be sure and tell the happiest
part of the story before I made an end) that many pleasant memories
greeted me as I came down the silent trail after a long absence. In the
distance sounded a lusty quacking; my imagination painted the mallards
at the end of the alder run, with sunshiny water and crimson bog and
misty-green larches around them, as a frame for the picture; and then
the whole beautiful anticipation came tumbling in ruin about my ears.

Before I reached my pond, before I saw the welcoming gleam of it even,
I was at every step going over my shoetops in water, where formerly I
had always found dry footing. Something disastrous had happened in my
absence; the whole bog was overflowed; around it was no mist of
delicate foliage but only skeleton trees, stark and pitiful. In my
heart I was berating the lumbermen, whose ugly works are the ruination
of every place they visit, when at last I waded to an opening that gave
outlook on my pond; and the first thing I noticed, as my eyes swept the
familiar scene, was a beaver-house cocked up on the shore, like a
warning sign of new ownership.

It is true that blessings brighten as they take their flight: not till
I read that crude sign of dispossession did I know how much pleasure my
little pond had given me. The lonely beauty which could quiet a man
like a psalm, or like an Indian’s wordless prayer; the glimpses of wild
creatures at home and unafraid; the succession of radiant pictures, at
sunny midday, or beneath the hushed twilight, or in the expectant
morning before the shadows come,—all these had suddenly taken wing,
driven away by mud-grubbing animals with a notion in their dull heads
that they wanted deeper water about the site they had chosen for their
house of sticks. It was too bad, too hopeless! I might have prevented
the ruin had I known; but now it was beyond all remedy. With a
different interest, therefore, and still resentful that my pond was
spoiled as thoroughly as any lumberman would have spoiled it, I made my
way around the flood to examine the beavers’ work at the outlet.








XV

BEAVER WORK


Hidden among the larches at the lower end of my pond was a tiny
outgoing stream, which had proved hard to find when first I explored
the region, and almost impossible to follow afterward. Under a fallen
log, so weathered and mossy that it seemed part of the natural shore, a
volume of water escaped without ripple or murmur, wandering away under
bending grasses to lose itself in an alder swamp, where innumerable
channels offered it lingering passage. From the swamp it found its way,
creepingly, among brooding cedars to a little brook, which went singing
far down through the woods to Upper Pine Pond; and beyond that on the
farther side was a long dead-water, and then Pine Stream making its
tortuous way through an untraveled region to the Penobscot. The nearest
beavers, a colony of four lodges which I unearthed on a hidden branch
of Pine Stream, were twelve or fifteen miles away, as the water flowed;
yet over all that distance an exploring family had made its lonely way,
guided at every turn by the flavor of distant springs, till one after
another they crept under the fallen log and entered my pond, which was
solitary enough to satisfy even their pioneer instincts. They had first
picked a site for their new lodge, on a point overlooking the lower
half of the pond, and had then gone back to the outlet to raise the
water.

Their dam was a rare piece of wild engineering; so much I had to
confess, even while I wished that the beavers had chosen some other
place to display their craft. Finding where the water escaped, they
stopped the opening beneath the log, and made a bank of mud and
alder-brush above it. This bank was carried out a dozen feet or more on
either side of the stream, the ends being bent forward (toward the pond
above) so as to make a very fine concave arch. On a small or quiet
stream like this, beavers almost invariably build a straight dam; and
where swift water calls for a stronger or curving structure, they
present the convex side to the current; but here they had reversed both
rules, for some reason or impulse which I could not fathom,—except on
the improbable assumption that the animals could foresee the end of
their work from the beginning. The finished dam was an amazingly good
one, as you shall see; but whether it resulted from planning or happy
experiment or just following the water, only a certain old beaver could
tell.

Since there was no other outlet to my pond, the beavers were obliged to
build here; but the site was a poor one, the land being uniformly low
on all sides, and no sooner did they finish their dam than the rising
water flowed around both ends of it. To remedy this they pushed out a
curving wing from either end of their first arch, so that the line of
their dam was now a pretty triple-curve. Again and again the outgoing
water crept around the obstacle; each time the beavers added other
curving wings, now on this side, now on that, bending them steadily
forward till the top of their dam suggested the rim of an enormous
scallop-shell. Then, finding the water deep enough for their needs,
they thrust out a straight wing from either end of their dam, resting
their work on the slopes of two hillocks in the woods, some fifty yards
apart,—this in a straight line, or across the hinge of the
scallop-shell: if measured on the curves, their dam was three or four
times that length. Their next task was to build a lodge on the point
above; then they dug a canal through the bog to the nearest grove of
hardwood, and cut down a liberal part of the trees for their winter
supply of bark. The branches of these trees had been cut into
convenient lengths, floated through the canal, and stored in a great
food-pile in the deep water near the lodge.

When I found the dam, several deer (to judge from the tracks) were
already using the top of it as a runway in passing from the flooded
ground on one side of the pond to the other. From either end a
game-trail led upward along the shore, no longer following immemorial
paths over the bog, which was submerged with all its splendor of color,
but making a new and rougher way through the black growth. When I
followed one of these trails it led me completely around the pond,
going confidently till it neared the salt-lick, where it halted,
wavered and trickled out in aimless wanderings. There, where once the
ground was trodden smooth by many feet, was now no ground to be seen.
The precious spring, over which a thousand generations of deer had
lingered, had vanished in a dull waste of water. Twice I watched the
place from early morning till owls began to cry the twilight; in that
time only a few animals appeared, singly, at long intervals; and after
wandering about as if seeking something and finding it not, they
disappeared in the dusky woods.

And so I went away, looking for the last time sadly on the little pond,
as upon a place one has owned and loved, but which has passed into
other hands. Though the wild ducks still breed or gather there, it is
no longer the same. There is no restful spot from which to watch the
waters dance with the wind, or frown at the cloud, or smile at the
sunshine; the little larches are all dead beside their ancestors; the
carpet of colored moss is but a memory. When the beavers go to pioneer
a remoter spot, I shall break their dam and let the water return to its
ancient level. Then, if happily I live long enough for another fringe
of larches to grow, and another mossy rug to crimson under the waning
sun, perhaps it will be my pond once more.



                                THE END








NOTES


[1] For further example and analysis of the matter, see pp. 196–199.

[2] Crawford, Thinking Black (1904).

[3] I am speaking of starlings as they now appear in southern New
England. They were brought from Europe a few years ago, and are
multiplying at an alarming rate. They have formed some curious new
habits here; even their voices are very different from the voices of
starlings as I have heard them in Europe.

[4] In parts of the West, I am told, wolves often kill more than they
need. Formerly they fed on the abundant game and were wholly natural
animals; but their habits have changed with a changed environment. When
the game was destroyed by settlers or hunters the wolves began to feed
on domestic animals; and the descendants of these wolves, which killed
right and left in a crowding, excited herd of sheep or cattle, are now
said to kill deer wantonly when they have the chance. I cannot
personally verify the saying, and know not whether it rests on
exceptional or typical observation. In the North, where there are no
domestic animals, I have rarely known a timber-wolf to kill after his
hunger was satisfied.











        
            *** END OF THE PROJECT GUTENBERG EBOOK HOW ANIMALS TALK ***
        

    

Updated editions will replace the previous one—the old editions will
be renamed.

Creating the works from print editions not protected by U.S. copyright
law means that no one owns a United States copyright in these works,
so the Foundation (and you!) can copy and distribute it in the United
States without permission and without paying copyright
royalties. Special rules, set forth in the General Terms of Use part
of this license, apply to copying and distributing Project
Gutenberg™ electronic works to protect the PROJECT GUTENBERG™
concept and trademark. Project Gutenberg is a registered trademark,
and may not be used if you charge for an eBook, except by following
the terms of the trademark license, including paying royalties for use
of the Project Gutenberg trademark. If you do not charge anything for
copies of this eBook, complying with the trademark license is very
easy. You may use this eBook for nearly any purpose such as creation
of derivative works, reports, performances and research. Project
Gutenberg eBooks may be modified and printed and given away—you may
do practically ANYTHING in the United States with eBooks not protected
by U.S. copyright law. Redistribution is subject to the trademark
license, especially commercial redistribution.


START: FULL LICENSE

THE FULL PROJECT GUTENBERG LICENSE

PLEASE READ THIS BEFORE YOU DISTRIBUTE OR USE THIS WORK

To protect the Project Gutenberg™ mission of promoting the free
distribution of electronic works, by using or distributing this work
(or any other work associated in any way with the phrase “Project
Gutenberg”), you agree to comply with all the terms of the Full
Project Gutenberg™ License available with this file or online at
www.gutenberg.org/license.

Section 1. General Terms of Use and Redistributing Project Gutenberg™
electronic works

1.A. By reading or using any part of this Project Gutenberg™
electronic work, you indicate that you have read, understand, agree to
and accept all the terms of this license and intellectual property
(trademark/copyright) agreement. If you do not agree to abide by all
the terms of this agreement, you must cease using and return or
destroy all copies of Project Gutenberg™ electronic works in your
possession. If you paid a fee for obtaining a copy of or access to a
Project Gutenberg™ electronic work and you do not agree to be bound
by the terms of this agreement, you may obtain a refund from the person
or entity to whom you paid the fee as set forth in paragraph 1.E.8.

1.B. “Project Gutenberg” is a registered trademark. It may only be
used on or associated in any way with an electronic work by people who
agree to be bound by the terms of this agreement. There are a few
things that you can do with most Project Gutenberg™ electronic works
even without complying with the full terms of this agreement. See
paragraph 1.C below. There are a lot of things you can do with Project
Gutenberg™ electronic works if you follow the terms of this
agreement and help preserve free future access to Project Gutenberg™
electronic works. See paragraph 1.E below.

1.C. The Project Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation (“the
Foundation” or PGLAF), owns a compilation copyright in the collection
of Project Gutenberg™ electronic works. Nearly all the individual
works in the collection are in the public domain in the United
States. If an individual work is unprotected by copyright law in the
United States and you are located in the United States, we do not
claim a right to prevent you from copying, distributing, performing,
displaying or creating derivative works based on the work as long as
all references to Project Gutenberg are removed. Of course, we hope
that you will support the Project Gutenberg™ mission of promoting
free access to electronic works by freely sharing Project Gutenberg™
works in compliance with the terms of this agreement for keeping the
Project Gutenberg™ name associated with the work. You can easily
comply with the terms of this agreement by keeping this work in the
same format with its attached full Project Gutenberg™ License when
you share it without charge with others.

1.D. The copyright laws of the place where you are located also govern
what you can do with this work. Copyright laws in most countries are
in a constant state of change. If you are outside the United States,
check the laws of your country in addition to the terms of this
agreement before downloading, copying, displaying, performing,
distributing or creating derivative works based on this work or any
other Project Gutenberg™ work. The Foundation makes no
representations concerning the copyright status of any work in any
country other than the United States.

1.E. Unless you have removed all references to Project Gutenberg:

1.E.1. The following sentence, with active links to, or other
immediate access to, the full Project Gutenberg™ License must appear
prominently whenever any copy of a Project Gutenberg™ work (any work
on which the phrase “Project Gutenberg” appears, or with which the
phrase “Project Gutenberg” is associated) is accessed, displayed,
performed, viewed, copied or distributed:

    This eBook is for the use of anyone anywhere in the United States and most
    other parts of the world at no cost and with almost no restrictions
    whatsoever. You may copy it, give it away or re-use it under the terms
    of the Project Gutenberg License included with this eBook or online
    at www.gutenberg.org. If you
    are not located in the United States, you will have to check the laws
    of the country where you are located before using this eBook.
  
1.E.2. If an individual Project Gutenberg™ electronic work is
derived from texts not protected by U.S. copyright law (does not
contain a notice indicating that it is posted with permission of the
copyright holder), the work can be copied and distributed to anyone in
the United States without paying any fees or charges. If you are
redistributing or providing access to a work with the phrase “Project
Gutenberg” associated with or appearing on the work, you must comply
either with the requirements of paragraphs 1.E.1 through 1.E.7 or
obtain permission for the use of the work and the Project Gutenberg™
trademark as set forth in paragraphs 1.E.8 or 1.E.9.

1.E.3. If an individual Project Gutenberg™ electronic work is posted
with the permission of the copyright holder, your use and distribution
must comply with both paragraphs 1.E.1 through 1.E.7 and any
additional terms imposed by the copyright holder. Additional terms
will be linked to the Project Gutenberg™ License for all works
posted with the permission of the copyright holder found at the
beginning of this work.

1.E.4. Do not unlink or detach or remove the full Project Gutenberg™
License terms from this work, or any files containing a part of this
work or any other work associated with Project Gutenberg™.

1.E.5. Do not copy, display, perform, distribute or redistribute this
electronic work, or any part of this electronic work, without
prominently displaying the sentence set forth in paragraph 1.E.1 with
active links or immediate access to the full terms of the Project
Gutenberg™ License.

1.E.6. You may convert to and distribute this work in any binary,
compressed, marked up, nonproprietary or proprietary form, including
any word processing or hypertext form. However, if you provide access
to or distribute copies of a Project Gutenberg™ work in a format
other than “Plain Vanilla ASCII” or other format used in the official
version posted on the official Project Gutenberg™ website
(www.gutenberg.org), you must, at no additional cost, fee or expense
to the user, provide a copy, a means of exporting a copy, or a means
of obtaining a copy upon request, of the work in its original “Plain
Vanilla ASCII” or other form. Any alternate format must include the
full Project Gutenberg™ License as specified in paragraph 1.E.1.

1.E.7. Do not charge a fee for access to, viewing, displaying,
performing, copying or distributing any Project Gutenberg™ works
unless you comply with paragraph 1.E.8 or 1.E.9.

1.E.8. You may charge a reasonable fee for copies of or providing
access to or distributing Project Gutenberg™ electronic works
provided that:

    • You pay a royalty fee of 20% of the gross profits you derive from
        the use of Project Gutenberg™ works calculated using the method
        you already use to calculate your applicable taxes. The fee is owed
        to the owner of the Project Gutenberg™ trademark, but he has
        agreed to donate royalties under this paragraph to the Project
        Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation. Royalty payments must be paid
        within 60 days following each date on which you prepare (or are
        legally required to prepare) your periodic tax returns. Royalty
        payments should be clearly marked as such and sent to the Project
        Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation at the address specified in
        Section 4, “Information about donations to the Project Gutenberg
        Literary Archive Foundation.”
    
    • You provide a full refund of any money paid by a user who notifies
        you in writing (or by e-mail) within 30 days of receipt that s/he
        does not agree to the terms of the full Project Gutenberg™
        License. You must require such a user to return or destroy all
        copies of the works possessed in a physical medium and discontinue
        all use of and all access to other copies of Project Gutenberg™
        works.
    
    • You provide, in accordance with paragraph 1.F.3, a full refund of
        any money paid for a work or a replacement copy, if a defect in the
        electronic work is discovered and reported to you within 90 days of
        receipt of the work.
    
    • You comply with all other terms of this agreement for free
        distribution of Project Gutenberg™ works.
    

1.E.9. If you wish to charge a fee or distribute a Project
Gutenberg™ electronic work or group of works on different terms than
are set forth in this agreement, you must obtain permission in writing
from the Project Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation, the manager of
the Project Gutenberg™ trademark. Contact the Foundation as set
forth in Section 3 below.

1.F.

1.F.1. Project Gutenberg volunteers and employees expend considerable
effort to identify, do copyright research on, transcribe and proofread
works not protected by U.S. copyright law in creating the Project
Gutenberg™ collection. Despite these efforts, Project Gutenberg™
electronic works, and the medium on which they may be stored, may
contain “Defects,” such as, but not limited to, incomplete, inaccurate
or corrupt data, transcription errors, a copyright or other
intellectual property infringement, a defective or damaged disk or
other medium, a computer virus, or computer codes that damage or
cannot be read by your equipment.

1.F.2. LIMITED WARRANTY, DISCLAIMER OF DAMAGES - Except for the “Right
of Replacement or Refund” described in paragraph 1.F.3, the Project
Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation, the owner of the Project
Gutenberg™ trademark, and any other party distributing a Project
Gutenberg™ electronic work under this agreement, disclaim all
liability to you for damages, costs and expenses, including legal
fees. YOU AGREE THAT YOU HAVE NO REMEDIES FOR NEGLIGENCE, STRICT
LIABILITY, BREACH OF WARRANTY OR BREACH OF CONTRACT EXCEPT THOSE
PROVIDED IN PARAGRAPH 1.F.3. YOU AGREE THAT THE FOUNDATION, THE
TRADEMARK OWNER, AND ANY DISTRIBUTOR UNDER THIS AGREEMENT WILL NOT BE
LIABLE TO YOU FOR ACTUAL, DIRECT, INDIRECT, CONSEQUENTIAL, PUNITIVE OR
INCIDENTAL DAMAGES EVEN IF YOU GIVE NOTICE OF THE POSSIBILITY OF SUCH
DAMAGE.

1.F.3. LIMITED RIGHT OF REPLACEMENT OR REFUND - If you discover a
defect in this electronic work within 90 days of receiving it, you can
receive a refund of the money (if any) you paid for it by sending a
written explanation to the person you received the work from. If you
received the work on a physical medium, you must return the medium
with your written explanation. The person or entity that provided you
with the defective work may elect to provide a replacement copy in
lieu of a refund. If you received the work electronically, the person
or entity providing it to you may choose to give you a second
opportunity to receive the work electronically in lieu of a refund. If
the second copy is also defective, you may demand a refund in writing
without further opportunities to fix the problem.

1.F.4. Except for the limited right of replacement or refund set forth
in paragraph 1.F.3, this work is provided to you ‘AS-IS’, WITH NO
OTHER WARRANTIES OF ANY KIND, EXPRESS OR IMPLIED, INCLUDING BUT NOT
LIMITED TO WARRANTIES OF MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR ANY PURPOSE.

1.F.5. Some states do not allow disclaimers of certain implied
warranties or the exclusion or limitation of certain types of
damages. If any disclaimer or limitation set forth in this agreement
violates the law of the state applicable to this agreement, the
agreement shall be interpreted to make the maximum disclaimer or
limitation permitted by the applicable state law. The invalidity or
unenforceability of any provision of this agreement shall not void the
remaining provisions.

1.F.6. INDEMNITY - You agree to indemnify and hold the Foundation, the
trademark owner, any agent or employee of the Foundation, anyone
providing copies of Project Gutenberg™ electronic works in
accordance with this agreement, and any volunteers associated with the
production, promotion and distribution of Project Gutenberg™
electronic works, harmless from all liability, costs and expenses,
including legal fees, that arise directly or indirectly from any of
the following which you do or cause to occur: (a) distribution of this
or any Project Gutenberg™ work, (b) alteration, modification, or
additions or deletions to any Project Gutenberg™ work, and (c) any
Defect you cause.

Section 2. Information about the Mission of Project Gutenberg™

Project Gutenberg™ is synonymous with the free distribution of
electronic works in formats readable by the widest variety of
computers including obsolete, old, middle-aged and new computers. It
exists because of the efforts of hundreds of volunteers and donations
from people in all walks of life.

Volunteers and financial support to provide volunteers with the
assistance they need are critical to reaching Project Gutenberg™’s
goals and ensuring that the Project Gutenberg™ collection will
remain freely available for generations to come. In 2001, the Project
Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation was created to provide a secure
and permanent future for Project Gutenberg™ and future
generations. To learn more about the Project Gutenberg Literary
Archive Foundation and how your efforts and donations can help, see
Sections 3 and 4 and the Foundation information page at www.gutenberg.org.

Section 3. Information about the Project Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation

The Project Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation is a non-profit
501(c)(3) educational corporation organized under the laws of the
state of Mississippi and granted tax exempt status by the Internal
Revenue Service. The Foundation’s EIN or federal tax identification
number is 64-6221541. Contributions to the Project Gutenberg Literary
Archive Foundation are tax deductible to the full extent permitted by
U.S. federal laws and your state’s laws.

The Foundation’s business office is located at 809 North 1500 West,
Salt Lake City, UT 84116, (801) 596-1887. Email contact links and up
to date contact information can be found at the Foundation’s website
and official page at www.gutenberg.org/contact

Section 4. Information about Donations to the Project Gutenberg
Literary Archive Foundation

Project Gutenberg™ depends upon and cannot survive without widespread
public support and donations to carry out its mission of
increasing the number of public domain and licensed works that can be
freely distributed in machine-readable form accessible by the widest
array of equipment including outdated equipment. Many small donations
($1 to $5,000) are particularly important to maintaining tax exempt
status with the IRS.

The Foundation is committed to complying with the laws regulating
charities and charitable donations in all 50 states of the United
States. Compliance requirements are not uniform and it takes a
considerable effort, much paperwork and many fees to meet and keep up
with these requirements. We do not solicit donations in locations
where we have not received written confirmation of compliance. To SEND
DONATIONS or determine the status of compliance for any particular state
visit www.gutenberg.org/donate.

While we cannot and do not solicit contributions from states where we
have not met the solicitation requirements, we know of no prohibition
against accepting unsolicited donations from donors in such states who
approach us with offers to donate.

International donations are gratefully accepted, but we cannot make
any statements concerning tax treatment of donations received from
outside the United States. U.S. laws alone swamp our small staff.

Please check the Project Gutenberg web pages for current donation
methods and addresses. Donations are accepted in a number of other
ways including checks, online payments and credit card donations. To
donate, please visit: www.gutenberg.org/donate.

Section 5. General Information About Project Gutenberg™ electronic works

Professor Michael S. Hart was the originator of the Project
Gutenberg™ concept of a library of electronic works that could be
freely shared with anyone. For forty years, he produced and
distributed Project Gutenberg™ eBooks with only a loose network of
volunteer support.

Project Gutenberg™ eBooks are often created from several printed
editions, all of which are confirmed as not protected by copyright in
the U.S. unless a copyright notice is included. Thus, we do not
necessarily keep eBooks in compliance with any particular paper
edition.

Most people start at our website which has the main PG search
facility: www.gutenberg.org.

This website includes information about Project Gutenberg™,
including how to make donations to the Project Gutenberg Literary
Archive Foundation, how to help produce our new eBooks, and how to
subscribe to our email newsletter to hear about new eBooks.